Attentional Load Attenuates Synaesthetic Priming Effects in Grapheme-Colour Synaesthesia

One of the hallmarks of grapheme-colour synaesthesia is that colours induced by letters, digits and words tend to interfere with the identification of coloured targets when the two colours are different, i.e., when they are incongruent. In a previous investigation (Mattingley et al., 2001) we found that this synaesthetic congruency effect occurs when an achromatic-letter prime precedes a coloured target, but that the effect disappears when the letter is pattern masked to prevent conscious recognition of its identity. Here we investigated whether selective attention modulates the synaesthetic congruency effect in a letter-priming task. Fourteen grapheme-colour synaesthetes and 14 matched, non-synaesthetic controls participated. The amount of selective attention available to process the letter-prime was limited by having participants perform a secondary visual task that involved discriminating pairs of gaps in adjacent limbs of a diamond surrounding the prime. In separate blocks of trials the attentional load of the secondary task was systematically varied to yield 'low load' and 'high load' conditions. We found a significant congruency effect for synaesthetes, but not for controls, when they performed a secondary attention-demanding task during presentation of the letter prime. Crucially, however, the magnitude of this priming was significantly reduced under conditions of high-load relative to low-load, indicating that attention plays an important role in modulating synaesthesia. Our findings help to explain the observation that synaesthetic colour experiences are often weak or absent during attention-demanding tasks.

[1]  K. A. Wollen,et al.  Colored-letter synesthesia. , 1983 .

[2]  Daniel Smilek,et al.  Seeing double: The role of meaning in alphanumeric-colour synaesthesia , 2003, Brain and Cognition.

[3]  C D Frith,et al.  Modulating irrelevant motion perception by varying attentional load in an unrelated task. , 1997, Science.

[4]  B. Laeng,et al.  Does Color Synesthesia Pose a Paradox for Early-Selection Theories of Attention? , 2004, Psychological science.

[5]  Derek Besner,et al.  Unconsciously controlled processing: the stroop effect reconsidered , 1999, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[6]  A Treisman,et al.  Feature binding, attention and object perception. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[7]  N. Lavie Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Visual attention: Insights from brain imaging , 2000, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[9]  Lynn C. Robertson,et al.  Synesthesia : perspectives from cognitive neuroscience , 2005 .

[10]  Thomas H Carr,et al.  Visual attention and word recognition in stroop color naming: is word recognition "automatic"? , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[11]  Randolph Blake,et al.  The perceptual reality of synesthetic colors , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[12]  J. Stroop Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .

[13]  A. Richa.,et al.  A systematic , large-scale study of synaesthesia : implications for the role of early experience in lexical-colour associations , 2005 .

[14]  Anina N. Rich,et al.  A systematic, large-scale study of synaesthesia: implications for the role of early experience in lexical-colour associations , 2005, Cognition.

[15]  Jason B. Mattingley,et al.  Unconscious priming eliminates automatic binding of colour and alphanumeric form in synaesthesia , 2001, Nature.

[16]  L. Robertson Binding, spatial attention and perceptual awareness , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[17]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Psychophysical investigations into the neural basis of synaesthesia , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[18]  Anina N. Rich,et al.  The effects of stimulus competition and voluntary attention on colour-graphemic synaesthesia , 2003, Neuroreport.

[19]  C. Brinkman,et al.  Do Synaesthetic Colours Act as Unique Features in Visual Search? , 2006, Cortex.

[20]  D. Besner,et al.  The myth of ballistic processing: Evidence from Stroop’s paradigm , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[21]  Y. Tsal,et al.  Perceptual load as a major determinant of the locus of selection in visual attention , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[22]  K L Shapiro,et al.  Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: an attentional blink? . , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[23]  Daniel Smilek,et al.  Five plus two equals yellow , 2000, Nature.

[24]  Jason B. Mattingley,et al.  Anomalous perception in synaesthesia: A cognitive neuroscience perspective , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[25]  J. H. Flowers,et al.  An Investigation of the Cognitive and Perceptual Dynamics of a Colour–Digit Synaesthete , 1999, Perception.

[26]  D. Smilek,et al.  Synaesthetic photisms guide attention , 2003, Brain and Cognition.

[27]  C. Mills,et al.  DIGIT SYNAESTHESIA: A CASE STUDY USING A STROOP-TYPE TEST , 1999 .

[28]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.