When Kinesthesia Becomes Visual: A Theoretical Justification for Executing Motor Tasks in Visual Space
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Anne C. Sittig,et al. The precision of proprioceptive position sense , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.
[2] S. Appelle. Perception and discrimination as a function of stimulus orientation: the "oblique effect" in man and animals. , 1972, Psychological bulletin.
[3] F. Rösler,et al. Interaction between gaze and visual and proprioceptive position judgements , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[4] Pietro G. Morasso,et al. Self-Organization, Computational Maps, and Motor Control , 1997 .
[5] Claire Leconte,et al. The plasticity of gravitational reference frame and the subjective vertical: Peripheral visual information affects the oblique effect , 2005, Neuroscience Letters.
[6] R. Sainburg,et al. The effect of target modality on visual and proprioceptive contributions to the control of movement distance , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[7] D. Goble,et al. Upper limb asymmetries in the matching of proprioceptive versus visual targets. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.
[8] S. Adamovich,et al. Pointing in 3D space to remembered targets. I. Kinesthetic versus visual target presentation. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.
[9] Astrid M. L. Kappers,et al. Intramanual and intermanual transfer of the curvature aftereffect , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.
[10] Alexandre Pouget,et al. A computational perspective on the neural basis of multisensory spatial representations , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[11] Erik J Schlicht,et al. Impact of coordinate transformation uncertainty on human sensorimotor control. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.
[12] Richard B. Ivry,et al. Intermanual interactions during initiation and production of rhythmic and discrete movements in individuals lacking a corpus callosum , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.
[13] Gabriele Polonara,et al. Contribution of posterior corpus callosum to the interhemispheric transfer of tactile information. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[14] Gunnar Blohm,et al. Multi-Sensory Weights Depend on Contextual Noise in Reference Frame Transformations , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[15] F. Lacquaniti,et al. Short-Term Memory for Reaching to Visual Targets: Psychophysical Evidence for Body-Centered Reference Frames , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[16] J. F. Soechting,et al. Transformation from Head- to Shoulder-Centered Representation of Target Direction in Arm Movements , 1990, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[17] J F Soechting,et al. A coordinate system for the synthesis of visual and kinesthetic information , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[18] Ian P. Howard,et al. Human visual orientation , 1982 .
[19] M. Mon-Williams,et al. Synaesthesia in the normal limb , 1997, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[20] Katja Fiehler,et al. A task-dependent effect of memory and hand-target on proprioceptive localization , 2012, Neuropsychologia.
[21] W Pieter Medendorp,et al. Behavioral reference frames for planning human reaching movements. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[22] M. Landy,et al. Weighted linear cue combination with possibly correlated error , 2003, Vision Research.
[23] Philip N. Sabes,et al. Sensory transformations and the use of multiple reference frames for reach planning , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.
[24] J. F. Soechting,et al. Errors in pointing are due to approximations in sensorimotor transformations. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.
[25] Joseph McIntyre,et al. Central Processes Amplify and Transform Anisotropies of the Visual System in a Test of Visual–Haptic Coordination , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[26] Michele Tagliabue,et al. Necessity is the Mother of Invention: Reconstructing Missing Sensory Information in Multiple, Concurrent Reference Frames for Eye–Hand Coordination , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[27] Robert J. van Beers,et al. How humans combine simultaneous proprioceptive and visual position information , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.
[28] S. Jbabdi,et al. How can a Bayesian approach inform neuroscience? , 2012, The European journal of neuroscience.
[29] Christopher A. Buneo,et al. Direct visuomotor transformations for reaching , 2002, Nature.
[30] J. Crawford,et al. Gaze-Centered Remapping of Remembered Visual Space in an Open-Loop Pointing Task , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[31] A. Yuille,et al. Object perception as Bayesian inference. , 2004, Annual review of psychology.
[32] Philip N. Sabes,et al. Flexible strategies for sensory integration during motor planning , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[33] A. Pouget,et al. Multisensory spatial representations in eye-centered coordinates for reaching , 2002, Cognition.
[34] F. Lacquaniti,et al. Viewer-centered frame of reference for pointing to memorized targets in three-dimensional space. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.
[35] Michele Tagliabue,et al. Eye-hand coordination when the body moves: Dynamic egocentric and exocentric sensory encoding , 2012, Neuroscience Letters.
[36] J. Koenderink,et al. Intermanual and intramanual tactual grating discrimination , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.
[37] P Viviani,et al. Pointing to Kinesthetic Targets in Space , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[38] Philip N. Sabes,et al. Sensory integration for reaching: models of optimality in the context of behavior and the underlying neural circuits. , 2011, Progress in brain research.
[39] R. J. van Beers,et al. Integration of proprioceptive and visual position-information: An experimentally supported model. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.
[40] Gabriel Baud-Bovy,et al. Amplitude and direction errors in kinesthetic pointing , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[41] Erin K. Cressman,et al. Proprioceptive localization of the left and right hands , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[42] Daniel M Wolpert,et al. Computational principles of sensorimotor control that minimize uncertainty and variability , 2007, The Journal of physiology.
[43] Angelo Quattrini,et al. Posterior Corpus Callosum and Interhemispheric Transfer of Somatosensory Information: An fMRI and Neuropsychological Study of a Partially Callosotomized Patient , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[44] S. Adamovich,et al. Pointing in 3D space to remembered targets II: Effects of movement speed toward kinesthetically defined targets , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[45] J. F. Soechting,et al. Early stages in a sensorimotor transformation , 1992, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[46] A. Gordon,et al. Contribution of tactile information to accuracy in pointing movements , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.
[47] Michael I. Jordan,et al. Computational models of sensorimotor integration , 1997 .
[48] Konrad Paul Kording,et al. Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning , 2004, Nature.
[49] Intermanual transfer of somaesthetic information: A two-point discrimination experiment , 1996, Neuropsychologia.
[50] M. Ernst,et al. Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.