Localizing the deficit in a case of jargonaphasia

We report the case of a neologistic jargonaphasic and ask whether her target-related and abstruse neologisms are the result of a single deficit, which affects some items more severely than others, or two deficits: one to lexical access and the other to phonological encoding. We analyse both correct/incorrect performance and errors and apply both traditional and formal methods (maximum-likelihood estimation and model selection). All evidence points to a single deficit at the level of phonological encoding. Further characteristics are used to constrain the locus still further. V.S. does not show the type of length effect expected of a memory component, nor the pattern of errors associated with an articulatory deficit. We conclude that her neologistic errors can result from a single deficit at a level of phonological encoding that immediately follows lexical access where segments are represented in terms of their features. We do not conclude, however, that this is the only possible locus that will produce phonological errors in aphasia, or, indeed, jargonaphasia.

[1]  Jason W. Brown,et al.  Ukeleles, condessors, and fosetch: Varieties of aphasic jargon , 1985 .

[2]  Jane Marshall,et al.  Jargon aphasia: What have we learned? , 2006 .

[3]  S. Chiat,et al.  Phoneme frequency effects in jargon aphasia: A phonological investigation of nonword errors , 2003, Brain and Language.

[4]  A. Baddeley Human Memory: Theory and Practice, Revised Edition , 1990 .

[5]  G. Dell,et al.  A Case-Series Test of the Interactive Two-Step Model of Lexical Access: Evidence from Picture Naming. , 2006 .

[6]  B. Rapp,et al.  Discreteness and interactivity in spoken word production. , 2000, Psychological review.

[7]  Max Coltheart,et al.  Cognitive Neuropsychology , 2014, Scholarpedia.

[8]  M. Schwartz,et al.  ORIGINS OF NONWORD PHONOLOGICAL ERRORS IN APHASIC PICTURE NAMING , 2004, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[9]  Gary S. Dell,et al.  The retrieval of phonological forms in production: tests of predictions from a connectionist model , 1988 .

[10]  Christopher Barry,et al.  Age of Acquisition, Word Frequency, and the Locus of Repetition Priming of Picture Naming , 2001 .

[11]  Stefanie Shattuck-Hufnagel,et al.  The Limited Use of Distinctive Features and Markedness in Speech Production: Evidence from Speech Error Data. , 1979 .

[12]  Andrew Olson,et al.  Patterns of Phonological Errors as a Function of a Phonological Versus an Articulatory Locus of Impairment , 2002, Cortex.

[13]  B. Rapp,et al.  Lexical and post-lexical phonological representations in spoken production , 2007, Cognition.

[14]  G. Dell,et al.  Stages of lexical access in language production , 1992, Cognition.

[15]  R. H. Baayen,et al.  The CELEX Lexical Database (CD-ROM) , 1996 .

[16]  H. Buckingham Where Do Neologisms Come From , 1981 .

[17]  Dan Gusfield,et al.  Algorithms on Strings, Trees, and Sequences - Computer Science and Computational Biology , 1997 .

[18]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The relation between syntactic and phonological knowledge in lexical access: evidence from the `tip-of-the-tongue' phenomenon , 1997, Cognition.

[19]  T. Alajouanine,et al.  VERBAL REALIZATION IN APHASIA , 1956 .

[20]  Ardi Roelofs,et al.  Phonological Segments and Features as Planning Units in Speech Production , 1999 .

[21]  D. Benson,et al.  Neologistic jargon: a clinicopathological study. , 1970, Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior.

[22]  J. Riddoch,et al.  Birmingham object recognition battery , 1993 .

[23]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[24]  Ardi Roelofs,et al.  Comprehension-Based Versus Production-Internal Feedback in Planning Spoken Words: A Rejoinder to Rapp and Goldrick (2004) , 2004 .

[25]  H. Akaike,et al.  Information Theory and an Extension of the Maximum Likelihood Principle , 1973 .

[26]  A. Kertesz,et al.  Neologistic jargon aphasia , 1976 .

[27]  Gregory V. Jones Deep dyslexia, imageability, and ease of predication , 1985, Brain and Language.

[28]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Converging evidence for the interaction of semantic and sublexical phonological information in accessing lexical representations for spoken output , 1995 .

[29]  Manjit,et al.  Neurology , 1912, NeuroImage.

[30]  Wolfram Ziegler,et al.  Syllable frequency and syllable structure in apraxia of speech , 2004, Brain and Language.

[31]  Trevor A. Harley,et al.  What causes a tip-of-the-tongue state? Evidence for lexical neighbourhood effects in speech production , 1998 .

[32]  W J Levelt,et al.  A case for the lemma/lexeme distinction in models of speaking: comment on Caramazza and Miozzo (1997) , 1998, Cognition.

[33]  Emer M.E. Forde and Glyn W. Humphreys A semantic locus for refractory behaviour : Implications for access-storage distinctions and the nature of semantic memory , 1997 .

[34]  J. Kay,et al.  An Effect of Imageability on the Production of Phonological Errors in Auditory Repetition , 1997 .

[35]  Elaine Funnell,et al.  Semantic Representation and Ease of Predication , 2000, Brain and Language.

[36]  W. Jason Owen,et al.  Statistical Data Analysis , 2000, Technometrics.

[37]  Brenda C Rapp,et al.  Feedback by Any Other Name Is Still Interactivity: A Reply to Roelofs (2004). , 2004 .

[38]  M. Coltheart,et al.  The cognitive neuropsychology of language , 1987 .

[39]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Computing the meanings of words in reading: cooperative division of labor between visual and phonological processes. , 2004, Psychological review.

[40]  H. Buckingham The Conduction Theory and Neologistic Jargon , 1977, Language and speech.

[41]  W. Levelt,et al.  Word frequency effects in speech production: Retrieval of syntactic information and of phonological form , 1994 .

[42]  S Dehaene,et al.  Competition between past and present. Assessment and interpretation of verbal perseverations. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[43]  Roger S. Brown,et al.  The "Tip of the Tongue" Phenomenon , 1966 .

[44]  S. Kohn,et al.  Distinctions between two phonological output deficits , 1994, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[45]  E. Kaplan,et al.  The Boston naming test , 2001 .

[46]  Jamie Ward,et al.  Serial position effects and lexical activation in spelling: Evidence from a single case study , 1998 .

[47]  D. Schacter,et al.  On the Tip of the Tongue An Event-Related fMRI Study of Semantic Retrieval Failure and Cognitive Conflict , 2001, Neuron.

[48]  G S Dell,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. , 1986, Psychological review.

[49]  Joseph Paul Stemberger,et al.  The nature of segments in the lexicon: Evidence from speech errors☆ , 1982 .

[50]  Luigi Burzio,et al.  Surface-to-Surface Morphology: when your Representations turn into Constraints , 1999 .

[51]  David R. Anderson,et al.  Model selection and multimodel inference : a practical information-theoretic approach , 2003 .

[52]  K. Patterson,et al.  The emergence of jargon in progressive fluent dysgraphia: The widening gap between target and response , 2001, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[53]  L. Nickels,et al.  Disentangling the Web: Neologistic Perseverative Errors in Jargon Aphasia , 2004, Neurocase.

[54]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Hesitation and the production of verbal paraphasias and neologisms in jargon aphasia , 1979, Brain and Language.

[55]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Mechanisms for accessing lexical representations for output: Evidence from a category-specific semantic deficit , 1991, Brain and Language.

[56]  G. Dell,et al.  Evidence for the involvement of a nonlexical route in the repetition of familiar words: A comparison of single and dual route models of auditory repetition , 2004, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[57]  L. Dehaene,et al.  Neologistic Jargon Sparing Numbers: A Category-specific Phonological Impairment , 1997 .

[58]  John J. Ohala,et al.  There is no interface between phonology and phonetics: a personal view , 1990 .

[59]  A. Paivio Dual coding theory: Retrospect and current status. , 1991 .

[60]  A. Jacobs,et al.  Patterns of phoneme and syllable frequency in jargon aphasia , 2005, Brain and Language.

[61]  C. Romani,et al.  Syllabic Constraints in the Phonological Errors of an Aphasic Patient , 1998, Brain and Language.

[62]  W. Levelt,et al.  Do speakers have access to a mental syllabary? , 1994, Cognition.

[63]  A. Caramazza How many levels of processing are there in lexical access , 1997 .

[64]  J. Hanley,et al.  Are Reading and Spelling Phonologically Mediated? Evidence from a Patient with a Speech Production Impairment , 1997 .

[65]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  The Sound Pattern of English , 1968 .

[66]  T Shallice,et al.  THE SELECTIVE IMPAIRMENT OF THE PHONOLOGICAL OUTPUT BUFFER , 2000, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[67]  M. McCloskey,et al.  Grapheme-to-lexeme feedback in the spelling system: Evidence from a dysgraphic patient , 2006, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[68]  Willem J. M. Levelt,et al.  A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[69]  J. Goldsmith Autosegmental and Metrical Phonology , 1990 .

[70]  Michael Wilson,et al.  MRC psycholinguistic database: Machine-usable dictionary, version 2.00 , 1988 .

[71]  M Coltheart,et al.  DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. , 2001, Psychological review.

[72]  G. Clements Papers in Laboratory Phonology: The role of the sonority cycle in core syllabification , 1990 .

[73]  A. Caramazza,et al.  On the distinction between deficits of access and deficits of storage : a question of theory , 1993 .

[74]  J. Hanley,et al.  Imageability effects, phonological errors, and the relationship between auditory repetition and picture naming: Implications for models of auditory repetition , 2002, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[75]  Cristina Romani,et al.  Effects of syllabic complexity in predicting accuracy of repetition and direction of errors in patients with articulatory and phonological difficulties , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[76]  Max Coltheart,et al.  Psycholinguistic assessments of language processing in aphasia (PALPA) , 1996 .

[77]  A. Lecours,et al.  A Phonological Investigation of Aphasic Speech, Sheila E. Blumstein. Mouton, The Hague (1973), 117 pp , 1975 .

[78]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The role of the (output) phonological buffer in reading, writing, and repetition , 1986 .