Associations with Minspeak icons.

UNLABELLED Although the Minspeak approach is used on communication devices worldwide, little research has been conducted on its applicability within specific cultural contexts. The impact that users' familiarity of symbols and associations can have on learnability necessitates more systematic research. This study was an investigation into the associations which young South African adults from a tertiary education institution make with selected Minspeak icons, used in Unity software. Associations with the 12 core icons of the UniChat program were elicited from 480 able-bodied tertiary education students, using a cueing questionnaire. Each participant completed one questionnaire on one icon, so that 40 response questionnaires were obtained per icon. The responses from the questionnaires were pruned and computerised. Two analyses were done from these data: Firstly, the percentage of common associations per icon was calculated, and secondly, the elicited associations were compared to the current Unity vocabulary on a word and conceptual level. The results indicate that some of the icons and their encoded vocabulary items might be used successfully in the South African context, while others need to be adapted to be locally relevant. The results emphasize that iconic encoding systems based on commercially available graphic representational systems cannot merely be imported to South Africa due to the multicultural and multilingual nature of the context. LEARNING OUTCOMES As a result of this activity (1) the participant will be able to understand different types of associational responses based on icons used in Minspeak; (2) the participant will be able to understand the cultural relevance of some aspects of the Unity application programme; and (3) the participant will be able to identify different strategies that can be used to describe the cultural relevance o of the Unity application programme.

[1]  A. Paton WAYS WITH WORDS , 1995 .

[2]  David S. Palermo Characteristics of Word Association Responses Obtained from Children in Grades One through Four. , 1971 .

[3]  A. Lehrer Semantic fields and lexical structure , 1974 .

[4]  Sheldon Richmond,et al.  Illusions, Patterns and Pictures: A Cross-Cultural Perspective by J. B. Deregowski (review) , 1983 .

[5]  M. B. Huer Examining perceptions of graphic symbols across cultures: Preliminary study of the impact of culture/ethnicity , 2000 .

[6]  J. Bruno Customizing a Minspeak1 system for a preliterate child: a case example , 1989 .

[7]  S. Heath Ways with Words: Language, Life and Work in Communities and Classrooms , 1983 .

[8]  N. Denzin,et al.  Handbook of Qualitative Research , 1994 .

[9]  B. Bernstein Theoretical Studies Towards a Sociology of Language , 1972 .

[10]  H. Pollio,et al.  SOME SEMANTIC RELATIONS AMONG WORD-ASSOCIATES. , 1964, The American journal of psychology.

[11]  Henry Goehl,et al.  Comparison of Picture and Word Association Performance in Adults and Preliterate Children , 1991 .

[12]  K. Nelson,et al.  The syntagmatic-paradigmatic shift revisited: a review of research and theory. , 1977, Psychological bulletin.

[13]  S. Petrey,et al.  Word associations and the development of lexical memory , 1977, Cognition.

[14]  W. Neuman,et al.  Social Research Methods: Qualitative and Quantitative Approaches , 2002 .

[15]  Helen Arvidson,et al.  Augmentative and Alternative Communication: New Directions in Research and Practice , 1999 .

[16]  Andrew W. Ellis,et al.  The Psychology of Language and Communication , 1985 .