Motor performance under stress: a test of the inverted-U hypothesis.

Investigated whether the relationship between arousal and motor performance among junior high school males supports the inverted U hypothesis. Based on the CMA scale, Ss were either low, medium, or high in trait anxiety and were subjected to 1 of 3 levels of psychological stress. A 3 × 3 factorial d

[1]  R. Martens Effect of an audience on learning and performance of a complex motor skill. , 1969, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[2]  R. Martens Palmar Sweating and the Presence of an Audience. , 1969 .

[3]  H. Leventhal,et al.  Palmar sweating: a quick and simple measure. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  James M. Dabbs,et al.  Enumeration of Active Sweat Glands: A Simple Physiological Indicator of Psychological Changes , 1967 .

[5]  E. Levitt The psychology of anxiety , 1967 .

[6]  R. Rosenthal Experimenter effects in behavioral research , 1968 .

[7]  K. Spence,et al.  CHAPTER 12 – The Motivational Components of Manifest Anxiety: Drive and Drive Stimuli1 , 1966 .

[8]  C. Spielberger Theory and research on anxiety , 1966 .

[9]  Charles D. Spielberger,et al.  Anxiety and behavior , 1966 .

[10]  J. Harrison,et al.  Physiological role of the adrenal medulla in the palmar anhidrotic response to stress. , 1966, Journal of applied physiology.

[11]  J. Hokanson,et al.  EFFECTS OF INDUCED MUSCULAR TENSION ON PERFORMANCE AND THE INVERTED U FUNCTION. , 1965, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[12]  Seymour Geisser,et al.  Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 1963 .

[13]  J. Harrison,et al.  Behaviour of the palmar sweat glands before and after operation. , 1962, Clinical science.

[14]  Elizabeth Duffy,et al.  Activation and behavior , 1962 .

[15]  W. E. Broen,et al.  A reaction potential ceiling and response decrements in complex situations. , 1961, Psychological review.

[16]  L. Pinneo,et al.  The effects of induced muscle tension during tracking on level of activation and on performance. , 1961, Journal of experimental psychology.

[17]  B. Martin,et al.  The assessment of anxiety by physiological behavioral measures. , 1961, Psychological bulletin.

[18]  R B MALMO,et al.  Activation: a neuropsychological dimension. , 1959, Rassegna giuliana di medicina.

[19]  M. F. Shore Perceptual efficiency as related to induced muscular effort and manifest anxiety. , 1958, Journal of experimental psychology.

[20]  E. Duffy,et al.  The psychological significance of the concept of arousal or activation. , 1957, Psychological review.

[21]  R B MALMO,et al.  Anxiety and behavioral arousal. , 1957, Psychological review.

[22]  R. Stennett,et al.  The relationship of performance level to level of arousal. , 1957, Journal of experimental psychology.

[23]  D. Palermo,et al.  The children's form of the manifest anxiety scale. , 1956, Child development.

[24]  J. Matarazzo,et al.  Anxiety level and pursuitmeter performance. , 1956, Journal of consulting psychology.

[25]  J. Matarazzo,et al.  Human Maze Performance as a Function of Increasing Levels of Anxiety , 1955 .

[26]  D. Hebb Drives and the C.N.S. (conceptual nervous system). , 1955, Psychological review.

[27]  J. Taylor,et al.  A personality scale of manifest anxiety. , 1953, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[28]  Sutarman,et al.  A new technique for enumerating active sweat glands in man. , 1952, The Journal of physiology.

[29]  J DEESE,et al.  The effects of psychological stress upon performance. , 1952, Psychological bulletin.

[30]  Frederick A. Courts Relations between muscular tension and performance. , 1942 .

[31]  G. L. Freeman The Facilitative and Inhibitory Effects of Muscular Tension upon Performance , 1933 .

[32]  J. T. Russell Relative Efficiency of Relaxation and Tension in Performing an Act of Skill , 1932 .

[33]  R. Yerkes,et al.  The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit‐formation , 1908 .