Infants' sensitivity to non-adjacent dependencies across phonological phrase boundaries.

Natural languages contain numerous non-adjacent relationships between words or morphemes in a sentence, often straddling phonological phrase boundaries (e.g., [these sheep] [have [ellipsis (horizontal)]]). Since phonological phrases are considered the main processing unit for infants, this may cause the acquisition of cross-phrase dependencies to be challenging. This study, however, shows that by 17 months of age, French-learning infants have nonetheless gained sensitivity to remote determiner-auxiliary co-occurrences that are interceded by phonological phrase boundaries. Infants thus possess a robust mechanism for tracking non-adjacent dependencies. This ability is essential for early grammatical development.

[1]  Rushen Shi,et al.  Syntactic Categorization in French-Learning Infants. , 2010, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[2]  J. Weissenborn,et al.  Functional Elements in Infants' Speech Processing: The Role of Determiners in the Syntactic Categorization of Lexical Elements , 2004 .

[3]  Anne Cutler,et al.  Recognition and Representation of Function Words in English-Learning Infants , 2006 .

[4]  J. Mehler,et al.  Perception of Prosodic Boundary Correlates by Newborn Infants. , 2001, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[5]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  The prosodic bootstrapping of phrases: Evidence from prelinguistic infants , 2003 .

[6]  Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al.  Gender-marked determiners help Dutch learners' word recognition when gender information itself does not. , 2011, Journal of child language.

[7]  J. Werker,et al.  Tuned to the signal: the privileged status of speech for young infants. , 2004, Developmental science.

[8]  Toben H. Mintz Frequent frames as a cue for grammatical categories in child directed speech , 2003, Cognition.

[9]  Rushen Shi,et al.  The effect of functional morphemes on word segmentation in preverbal infants. , 2008, Developmental science.

[10]  R. Gómez,et al.  A first step in form-based category abstraction by 12-month-old infants. , 2004, Developmental science.

[11]  J. Mehler,et al.  Phonological phrase boundaries constrain lexical access II. Infant data , 2004 .

[12]  Marieke van Heugten,et al.  Linking infants’ distributional learning abilities to natural language acquisition , 2010 .

[13]  Anne Cutler,et al.  Frequency and form as determinants of functor sensitivity in English-acquiring infants. , 2006, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[14]  J. Weissenborn,et al.  The Recognition of Discontinuous Verbal Dependencies by German 19-Month-Olds: Evidence for Lexical and Structural Influences on Children's Early Processing Capacities , 2006 .

[15]  Barbara Höhle,et al.  German-learning infants' ability to detect unstressed closed class elements in continuous speech , 2003 .

[16]  R. Gómez,et al.  The Developmental Trajectory of Nonadjacent Dependency Learning. , 2005, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[17]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Sensitivity to discontinuous dependencies in language learners: evidence for limitations in processing space , 1998, Cognition.

[18]  Rushen Shi,et al.  French-learning toddlers use gender information on determiners during word recognition. , 2009, Developmental science.

[19]  Marina Nespor,et al.  An interaction between prosody and statistics in the segmentation of fluent speech , 2007, Cognitive Psychology.

[20]  Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al.  Infants use prosodically conditioned acoustic-phonetic cues to extract words from speech. , 2008, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[21]  M. Goldsmith,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996 .