Symbolic distance between numerosity and identity modulates Stroop interference.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] W. Banks,et al. Semantic congruity effects in comparative judgments of magnitudes of digits. , 1976 .
[2] Gordon D. Logan,et al. On the relation between production and verification tasks in the psychology of simple arithmetic , 1990 .
[3] Jamie I. D. Campbell,et al. An encoding-complex view of cognitive number processing: Comment on McCloskey, Sokol, and Goodman (1986). , 1988 .
[4] J. Windes,et al. Reaction time for numerical coding and naming of numerals. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.
[5] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. Cognitive mechanisms in number processing and calculation: Evidence from dyscalculia , 1985, Brain and Cognition.
[6] J. Stroop. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .
[7] S. Dehaene,et al. The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .
[8] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Development of Elementary Numerical Abilities: A Neuronal Model , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[9] E. L. Kaufman,et al. The discrimination of visual number. , 1949, The American journal of psychology.
[10] F. Restle. Speed of Adding and Comparing Numbers. , 1970 .
[11] G. R. Potts,et al. Mental comparison of size and magnitude: size congruity effects. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[12] John M. Parkman,et al. Temporal aspects of digit and letter inequality judgments. , 1971, Journal of Experimental Psychology.
[13] Avishai Henik,et al. Automatic and intentional processing of numerical information , 1992 .
[14] W. Neill,et al. Selective attention and the inhibitory control of cognition. , 1995 .
[15] S. Poltrock,et al. A random walk model of digit comparison , 1989 .
[16] B. Forrin,et al. Naming latency and the repetition of stimulus categories , 1974 .
[17] G. S. Klein,et al. SEMANTIC POWER MEASURED THROUGH THE INTERFERENCE OF WORDS WITH COLOR-NAMING. , 1964, The American journal of psychology.
[18] G. Logan,et al. Memory-based automaticity in the discrimination of visual numerosity. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[19] S. Tipper. The Negative Priming Effect: Inhibitory Priming by Ignored Objects , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[20] R. Moyer. Comparing objects in memory: Evidence suggesting an internal psychophysics , 1973 .
[21] C. Umilta,et al. Further evidence on the effects of symbolic distance on Stroop-like interference , 1999 .
[22] G D Logan,et al. On the autonomy of mental processes: a case study of arithmetic. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[23] D. A. Taylor,et al. The cuing and priming of cognitive operations. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[24] P. Shaver,et al. Color association values and response interference on variants of the Stroop test. , 1967, Acta psychologica.
[25] A. Henik,et al. Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks , 1982, Memory & cognition.
[26] Charles J. Brainerd,et al. New perspectives on interference and inhibition in cognition: Final comments , 1995 .
[27] Verbal interference with perceptual classification: The effect of semantic structure , 1973 .
[28] S. Dehaene,et al. Attention, automaticity, and levels of representation in number processing , 1995 .
[29] Dale S. Klopfer,et al. Stroop Interference and Color-Word Similarity , 1996 .
[30] R E Morin,et al. Recognition memory and reaction time. , 1967, Acta psychologica.
[31] S. Dehaene. Varieties of numerical abilities , 1992, Cognition.
[32] A. T. Welford,et al. THE MEASUREMENT OF SENSORY-MOTOR PERFORMANCE : SURVEY AND REAPPRAISAL OF TWELVE YEARS' PROGRESS , 1960 .
[33] Z. Pylyshyn,et al. Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently? A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. , 1994, Psychological review.
[34] J. Morton,et al. Categories of interference: verbal mediation and conflict in card sorting. , 1969, British journal of psychology.
[35] F. Campbell,et al. The Magic Number 4 ± 0: A New Look at Visual Numerosity Judgements , 1976, Perception.
[36] E. M. Jensen,et al. The subitizing and counting of visually presented fields of dots. , 1950 .
[37] Robert Sekuler,et al. Processing numerical information: A choice time analysis , 1971 .
[38] K. Briand,et al. Priming single digit numbers: automatic spreading activation dissipates as a function of semantic distance , 1986 .
[39] C. Gallistel,et al. The Child's Understanding of Number , 1979 .
[40] William P. Banks,et al. Encoding and Processing of Symbolic Information in Comparative Judgments1 , 1977 .
[41] James V. Hinrichs,et al. Two-digit number comparison: Use of place information. , 1981 .
[42] Ronald E. Shor,et al. Semantic gradients and interference in naming color, spatial direction, and numerosity , 1971 .
[43] Z. Pylyshyn,et al. What enumeration studies can show us about spatial attention: evidence for limited capacity preattentive processing. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[44] Matthew Flatt,et al. PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers , 1993 .
[45] R. Moyer,et al. Mental comparison and the symbolic distance effect , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.
[46] S. Tipper,et al. Behavioural Goals Determine Inhibitory Mechanisms of Selective Attention , 1994 .
[47] Robert Sekuler,et al. Children's Judgments of Numerical Inequality. , 1977 .
[48] W. Trammell Neill,et al. Selective attention and the suppression of cognitive noise. , 1987 .
[49] ROBERT S. MOYER,et al. Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.
[50] J. L. Warner,et al. Response and encoding factors in “ignoring” irrelevant information , 1979 .
[51] M. Brysbaert. Arabic number reading: On the nature of the numerical scale and the origin of phonological recoding. , 1995 .
[52] C M Francolini,et al. On the nonautomaticity of “automatic” activation: Evidence of selective seeing , 1980, Perception & psychophysics.
[53] R. Shor,et al. Symbol Processing Speed Differences and Symbol Interference Effects in a Variety of Concept Domains. , 1971, The Journal of general psychology.
[54] Colin M. Macleod. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.
[55] S. Dehaene,et al. Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[56] H Egeth,et al. Subitizing: Direct apprehension or serial processing? , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.
[57] G. Mandler,et al. Subitizing: an analysis of its component processes. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[58] E. M. Duncan,et al. Isolating the effects of symbolic distance, and semantic congruity in comparative judgments: An additive-factors analysis , 1980, Memory & cognition.
[59] R W Proctor,et al. Sources of color-word interference in the Stroop color-naming task , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.
[60] G. M. Redding,et al. Stroop Effect: Interference and Facilitation with Verbal and Manual Responses , 1977, Perceptual and motor skills.