Selective attention to real-world objects drives their emotional appraisal
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Craig S. Chapman,et al. Models, movements, and minds: bridging the gap between decision making and action , 2020, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[2] Agnieszka M. Nowik,et al. The neural underpinnings of haptically guided functional grasping of tools: An fMRI study , 2019, NeuroImage.
[3] Daniel Baldauf,et al. Visual Selection of the Future Reach Path in Obstacle Avoidance , 2018, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[4] M. Fenske,et al. Affective evidence that inhibition is involved in separating accessory representations from active representations in visual working memory , 2018, Visual Cognition.
[5] Michel F. Failing,et al. Selection history: How reward modulates selectivity of visual attention , 2017, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[6] M. Fenske,et al. Neural evidence that inhibition is linked to the affective devaluation of distractors that match the contents of working memory , 2017, Neuropsychologia.
[7] Craig S. Chapman,et al. Reaching reveals that best-versus-rest processing contributes to biased decision making. , 2017, Acta psychologica.
[8] A. Dijksterhuis,et al. How does not responding to appetitive stimuli cause devaluation: Evaluative conditioning or response inhibition? , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[9] B. Anderson. The attention habit: how reward learning shapes attentional selection , 2016, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[10] Seth B. Agyei,et al. Development of Visual Motion Perception for Prospective Control: Brain and Behavioral Studies in Infants , 2016, Front. Psychol..
[11] Craig S. Chapman,et al. The snooze of lose: Rapid reaching reveals that losses are processed more slowly than gains. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[12] Z. Estes,et al. Touch and go: merely grasping a product facilitates brand perception and choice , 2015 .
[13] Jacqueline C. Snow,et al. Real-world objects are more memorable than photographs of objects , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[14] Jason P. Gallivan,et al. Three-dimensional reach trajectories as a probe of real-time decision-making between multiple competing targets , 2014, Front. Neurosci..
[15] A. Gallace,et al. Effects of Arm Crossing on Spatial Perspective-Taking , 2014, PloS one.
[16] P. Gollwitzer,et al. Distractor devaluation in a flanker task: object-specific effects without distractor recognition memory. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[17] Ashleigh M. Hover,et al. Changing value through cued approach: An automatic mechanism of behavior change , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.
[18] Anna Schubö,et al. Modulation of visual attention by object affordance , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[19] W. Stroebe,et al. Stop signals decrease choices for palatable foods through decreased food evaluation , 2013, Front. Psychol..
[20] L. Chelazzi,et al. Rewards teach visual selective attention , 2013, Vision Research.
[21] H. Aarts,et al. Using stop signals to inhibit chronic dieters' responses toward palatable foods. , 2011, Behaviour research and therapy.
[22] Jody C. Culham,et al. Bringing the real world into the fMRI scanner: Repetition effects for pictures versus real objects , 2011, Scientific reports.
[23] Patryk A. Laurent,et al. Value-driven attentional capture , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[24] Jody C. Culham,et al. Mental blocks: fMRI reveals top-down modulation of early visual cortex when obstacles interfere with grasp planning , 2011, Neuropsychologia.
[25] Ian Krajbich,et al. Visual fixations and the computation and comparison of value in simple choice , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.
[26] D. Lloyd,et al. Right hand presence modulates shifts of exogenous visuospatial attention in near perihand space , 2010, Brain and Cognition.
[27] H. Deubel,et al. Attentional landscapes in reaching and grasping , 2010, Vision Research.
[28] S. Tipper,et al. Sensorimotor fluency influences affect: Evidence from electromyography , 2010 .
[29] J. Peck,et al. The Effect of Mere Touch on Perceived Ownership , 2009 .
[30] Brian A. Goolsby,et al. Distractor devaluation requires visual working memory , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[31] Oren Griffiths,et al. Negative priming reduces affective ratings , 2008 .
[32] S. Tipper,et al. Self produced and observed actions influence emotion: the roles of action fluency and eye gaze , 2008, Psychological research.
[33] Harm Veling,et al. When approach motivation and behavioral inhibition collide: Behavior regulation through stimulus devaluation , 2018 .
[34] Daniel N. Bub,et al. Language-Based Access to Gestural Components of Conceptual Knowledge , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[35] Denis G. Pelli,et al. ECVP '07 Abstracts , 2007, Perception.
[36] John G. Taylor,et al. Efficient Attentional Selection Predicts Distractor Devaluation: Event-related Potential Evidence for a Direct Link between Attention and Emotion , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[37] A. van Knippenberg,et al. Devaluation of distracting stimuli , 2007 .
[38] Jane E. Raymond,et al. Affective Influences of Selective Attention , 2006 .
[39] Heiner Deubel,et al. Deployment of visual attention before sequences of goal-directed hand movements , 2006, Vision Research.
[40] Jane E Raymond,et al. Emotional devaluation of distracting patterns and faces: a consequence of attentional inhibition during visual search? , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[41] Melina A. Kunar,et al. The affective consequences of visual attention in preview search , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[42] D. Elliott,et al. Movement Trajectories in the Presence of a Distracting Stimulus: Evidence for a Response Activation Model of Selective Reaching , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[43] Jane E Raymond,et al. Selective Attention Determines Emotional Responses to Novel Visual Stimuli , 2003, Psychological science.
[44] R. Compton,et al. Hemispheric Differences in Affective Response: Effects of Mere Exposure , 2002 .
[45] R. Johansson,et al. Eye–Hand Coordination in Object Manipulation , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[46] S. Tipper,et al. Selective Reaching to Grasp: Evidence for Distractor Interference Effects , 1997 .
[47] K Nakayama,et al. Visual attention to surfaces in three-dimensional space. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[48] Arthur F. Kramer,et al. Limits of focused attention in three-dimensional space , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.
[49] J. Cacioppo,et al. Rudimentary determinants of attitudes. II: Arm flexion and extension have differential effects on attitudes. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[50] S. Tipper,et al. Hand deviations away from visual cues: Indirect evidence for inhibition , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[51] Heiner Deubel,et al. Attentional selection in sequential manual movements, movements around an obstacle and in grasping , 2005 .
[52] M. Kinsbourne. Mechanisms of Unilateral Neglect , 1987 .
[53] J. Gibson. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .
[54] B. Tversky,et al. When Far Becomes Near: Perspective Taking Induces Social Remapping of Spatial Relations , 2022 .