Postural instability induced by virtual reality exposure: Development of a certification protocol

Exposure to virtual environments often causes users to experience symptoms of motion sickness. An accessory manifestation of motion sickness symptoms is postural disequilibrium. If the postural disequilibrium that occurs persists beyond the time an individual is within the confines of the laboratory or system site, user safety could be compromised and products liability issues could be forthcoming. In this study, a portable, automated postural assessment system is developed that can be employed before and after exposure to a virtual reality (VR) system in order to certify that a user's balance on exiting the system is at least demonstrably as good as it was on entering. It is argued that if the "coming out" balance performance is sufficiently poorer than the "going in" balance, then the user should be retained until the pretest balance performance is regained. The results from a set of normative and validation experiments on postural equilibrium identified several reliable measures of stance that could serve as a basis for certification. Furthermore, a new automated video-based measure using only head movement showed that performance over sessions is stable and reliable. The head movement changes that occur with stimuli, such as alcohol and simulator exposure, are well behaved, predictable, and significant even with small samples. The implication is that the proposed objective measure of postural stability, in conjunction with procedures for obtaining self-reports of symptoms, can afford some measure of certification that exposure to a given VR system is without harm.

[1]  B. J. Winer Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 1992 .

[2]  R S Kennedy,et al.  Simulator sickness in U.S. Navy flight simulators. , 1989, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[3]  F E Guedry,et al.  Habituation to Complex Vestibular Stimulation in Man: Transfer and Retention of Effects from Twelve Days of Rotation at 10 Rpm , 1965, Perceptual and motor skills.

[4]  Rudy Darken,et al.  Navigating large virtual spaces , 1996, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact..

[5]  M Bergstedt,et al.  Relationships between blood alcohol, positional alcohol nystagmus and postural equilibrium. , 1967, Quarterly journal of studies on alcohol.

[6]  R. Kennedy,et al.  EFFECTS OF EXPOSURE TO A ROTATING ENVIRONMENT (10 RPM) ON FOUR AVIATORS FOR A PERIOD OF TWELVE DAYS. , 1965, Aerospace medicine.

[7]  Goodson Je,et al.  Motion sickness in a helicopter simulator. , 1960 .

[8]  Gary E. Riccio,et al.  Visually Induced Motion Sickness in Virtual Environments , 1992, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[9]  A. Graybiel,et al.  Sopite syndrome: a sometimes sole manifestation of motion sickness. , 1976, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[10]  G. H. Crampton,et al.  Motion and Space Sickness , 1990 .

[11]  Randy F. Pausch,et al.  A Literature Survey for Virtual Environments: Military Flight Simulator Visual Systems and Simulator Sickness , 1992, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[12]  T. Stoffregen,et al.  An ecological Theory of Motion Sickness and Postural Instability , 1991 .

[13]  C. S. Harris Perceptual adaptation to inverted, reversed, and displaced vision. , 1965, Psychological review.

[14]  Ernest A. Kallman,et al.  Ethical Evaluation: A Necessary Element in Virtual Environment Research , 1993, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[15]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  The role of vision in the control of posture during linear motion. , 1979, Progress in brain research.

[16]  H E Ross,et al.  Adaptation of divers to curvature distortion under water. , 1970, Ergonomics.

[17]  H C Diener,et al.  On the role of vestibular, visual and somatosensory information for dynamic postural control in humans. , 1988, Progress in brain research.

[18]  D. Whitteridge,et al.  Control of eye movement , 1978, Nature.

[19]  R. Kennedy,et al.  COMPARATIVE EFFECTS OF PROLONGED ROTATION AT 10 RPM ON POSTURAL EQUILIBRIUM IN VESTIBULAR NORMAL AND VESTIBULAR DEFECTIVE HUMAN SUBJECTS. NSAM-920. , 1965, Research report. Naval School of Aviation Medicine.

[20]  Margaret D. Nolan,et al.  Vection and simulator sickness. , 1990, Military psychology : the official journal of the Division of Military Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[21]  Phillip A. K. Hash,et al.  Control : a primary driver of cybersickness , 1995 .

[22]  J R Lackner,et al.  Postural illusions experienced during Z-axis recumbent rotation and their dependence upon somatosensory stimulation of the body surface. , 1978, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[23]  M F Reschke,et al.  Recovery of Postural Equilibrium Control following Spaceflight a , 1992, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[24]  R. Welch Perceptual Modification: Adapting to Altered Sensory Environments , 1978 .

[25]  J. Paillard,et al.  Active and passive movements in the calibration of position sense , 1968 .

[26]  R. Kennedy,et al.  Profile analysis of after-effects experienced during exposure to several virtual reality environments , 1994 .

[27]  R S Kennedy,et al.  Symptomatology under storm conditions in the North Atlantic in control subjects and in persons with bilateral labyrinthine defects. , 1968, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[28]  A GRAYBIEL,et al.  MOTION SICKNESS SYMPTOMATOLOGY OF LABYRINTHINE DEFECTIVE AND NORMAL SUBJECTS DURING ZERO GRAVITY MANEUVERS. , 1964, Aerospace medicine.

[29]  Kamran Barin,et al.  Effect of Head Orientation on the Diagnostic Sensitivity of Posturography in Patients with Compensated Unilateral Lesions , 1992, Otolaryngology--head and neck surgery : official journal of American Academy of Otolaryngology-Head and Neck Surgery.

[30]  Daniel W. Gower,et al.  The time course of postflight simulator sickness symptoms. , 1989, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[31]  J. W. Miller,et al.  Motion sickness in a helicopter simulator. , 1960, Aerospace medicine.

[32]  F O Black,et al.  Clinical application of dynamic posturography for evaluating sensory integration and vestibular dysfunction. , 1990, Neurologic clinics.

[33]  James R. Lackner,et al.  Spatial Orientation, Adaptation, and Motion Sickness in Real and Virtual Environments , 1992, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[34]  A Berthoz,et al.  The role of gaze in compensation of vestibular disfunction: the gaze substitution hypothesis. , 1988, Progress in brain research.

[35]  T. Stoffregen,et al.  Affordances as constraints on the control of stance , 1988 .

[36]  Sheldon M. Ebenholtz,et al.  Motion Sickness and Oculomotor Systems in Virtual Environments , 1992, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[37]  Gary E. Riccio,et al.  An Ecological Critique of the Sensory Conflict Theory of Motion Sickness , 1991 .

[38]  H. H. Kornhuber,et al.  The Vestibular System and the General Motor System , 1974 .

[39]  J. Dichgans,et al.  Visual-Vestibular Interaction: Effects on Self-Motion Perception and Postural Control , 1978 .

[40]  E Y Lambert,et al.  The Effects of Asynchronous Visual Delays on Simulator Flight Performance and the Development of Simulator Sickness Symptomatology , 1986 .