Actual and perceived social norms of children’s road crossing behavior

This study compared the actual and perceived social norms regarding road crossing behaviors, and aimed at uncovering a misperception of group norms. The basic notion underlying the study was that if children perceived the behaviors and attitudes of their friends regarding road crossing as more negative than they actually are, this might lead the children to adopt riskier behavior when crossing the road. The participants were comprised of 123 elementary school children (ages from 6 to 13; 78 boys – 65% and 42 girls – 35%) from Beitar-Ilit, a religious city in Israel. A questionnaire especially constructed for the study was administered to the participants. A t-test yielded a significant difference between the child’s own attitudes score and the perceived peers’ attitudes score. On average, children perceive their friends’ attitudes with regard to road crossing behaviors as being more negative compared to their own attitudes. A second t-test yielded a significant difference between the child’s own behavior score and the perceived peers’ behavior score. Children perceived the road crossing behavior of their friends as riskier relative to their own crossing behavior. A regression test revealed that the variables, perceived peers’ attitudes and perceived peers’ behaviors, contributed significantly to the explained variance: the more risky the perceived peers’ attitudes and behaviors with regard to road crossing, the more risky the child’s own behavior when crossing the road. It is hoped that an interventional educational program based on positive norms in the social group will encourage children to exhibit safe behavior in the roads.

[1]  M Suzanne Zeedyk,et al.  Stop, look, listen, and think? What young children really do when crossing the road. , 2002, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[2]  Jorn Thielmann,et al.  Islam and Muslims in Germany , 2008 .

[3]  T. Rothengatter,et al.  Classroom instruction versus roadside training in traffic safety education , 1997 .

[4]  Young children's conception of speed: possible implications for pedestrian safety , 1988 .

[5]  L. Lam Parental risk perceptions of childhood pedestrian road safety: a cross cultural comparison. , 2005, Journal of safety research.

[6]  C. Lewis,et al.  Because it's there! Why some children count, rather than infer numerical relationships , 2005 .

[7]  J. Thomson Developing safe route planning strategies in young child pedestrians , 1997 .

[8]  J L Schofer,et al.  Supervision in childhood injury cases: a reliable taxonomy. , 1997, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[9]  Tova Rosenbloom,et al.  Fear and danger appraisals of a road-crossing scenario: a developmental perspective. , 2008, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[10]  F. Heider The psychology of interpersonal relations , 1958 .

[11]  M Suzanne Zeedyk,et al.  Behavioural observations of adult-child pairs at pedestrian crossings. , 2003, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[12]  Max Bushell,et al.  Pedestrian crash trends and potential countermeasures from around the world. , 2012, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[13]  J. Thomson,et al.  Children's perception of safety and danger on the road. , 1991, British journal of psychology.

[14]  Michael R. Corbett,et al.  "Do as I say, not as I do": family influences on children's safety and risk behaviors. , 2008, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[15]  M. Larimer,et al.  Biases in the perception of drinking norms among college students. , 1991, Journal of studies on alcohol.

[16]  R B Isler,et al.  Child pedestrians' crossing gap thresholds. , 1998, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[17]  James A. Thomson Child development and the aims of road safety education : a review and analysis , 1996 .

[18]  J. Sweeney,et al.  The Social Norms of Tax Compliance: Evidence from Australia, Singapore, and the United States , 2007 .

[19]  Benjamin K Barton,et al.  The roles of age, gender, inhibitory control, and parental supervision in children's pedestrian safety. , 2006, Journal of pediatric psychology.

[20]  Deborah A. Prentice,et al.  Pluralistic ignorance and alcohol use on campus: some consequences of misperceiving the social norm. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[21]  Joan Severson,et al.  Validation of virtual reality as a tool to understand and prevent child pedestrian injury. , 2008, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[22]  H. Perkins,et al.  Engaging Men as Social Justice Allies in Ending Violence Against Women: Evidence for a Social Norms Approach , 2003, Journal of American college health : J of ACH.

[23]  Clayton Neighbors,et al.  Normative misperception and the impact of descriptive and injunctive norms on college student gambling. , 2003, Psychology of addictive behaviors : journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors.

[24]  H. Perkins,et al.  Perceiving the community norms of alcohol use among students: some research implications for campus alcohol education programming. , 1986, The International journal of the addictions.

[25]  Henry Wechsler,et al.  Variation in Perceived College Drinking Norms and its Impact on Alcohol Abuse: A Nationwide Study , 1996 .

[26]  Tova Rosenbloom,et al.  Social norms of accompanied young children and observed crossing behaviors. , 2009, Journal of safety research.

[27]  E. Pedersen,et al.  Assessment of perceived and actual alcohol norms in varying contexts: exploring Social Impact Theory among college students. , 2008, Addictive behaviors.

[28]  Tova Rosenbloom,et al.  For heaven’s sake follow the rules: pedestrians’ behavior in an ultra-orthodox and a non-orthodox city , 2004 .

[29]  H. Perkins Social norms and the prevention of alcohol misuse in collegiate contexts. , 2002, Journal of studies on alcohol. Supplement.