Journal of Experimental Psychology : Human Perception and Performance Spatial ( Mis-) Interpretation of Pointing Gestures to Distal Referents

Pointing gestures are a vital aspect of human communication. Nevertheless, observers consistently fail to determine the exact location to which another person points when that location lies in the distance. Here we explore the reasons for this misunderstanding. Humans usually point by extending the arm and finger. We show that observer's interpret these gestures by nonlinear extrapolation of the pointer's arm-finger line. The nonlinearity can be adequately described as the Bayesian-optimal integration of a linear extrapolation of the arm-finger line and observers' prior assumptions about likely referent positions. Surprisingly, the spatial rule describing the interpretation of pointing gestures differed from the rules describing the production of these gestures. In the latter case, the eye, index finger, and referent were aligned. We show that the differences in the production and interpretation of pointing gestures accounts for the systematic spatial misunderstanding of pointing gestures to distant referents. No evidence was found for the hypotheses that action-related processes are involved in the perception of pointing gestures. How participants interpreted pointing gestures was independent of how they produce these gestures and whether they had practiced pointing movements before. By contrast, both the production and interpretation seem to be primarily determined by salient visual cues.

[1]  D. Knill,et al.  The Bayesian brain: the role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.

[2]  Dejan Todorović,et al.  Geometrical basis of perception of gaze direction , 2006, Vision Research.

[3]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Infants use shared experience to interpret pointing gestures. , 2009, Developmental science.

[4]  John M Kennedy,et al.  Pivots for pointing: visually-monitored pointing has higher arm elevations than pointing blindfolded. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  R. Passingham,et al.  Action observation and acquired motor skills: an FMRI study with expert dancers. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  Jacqueline M. Fulvio,et al.  Visual extrapolation of contour geometry. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[7]  Frederik Hegger,et al.  Johnny: An Autonomous Service Robot for Domestic Environments , 2011, Journal of Intelligent & Robotic Systems.

[8]  P. Thier,et al.  How precise is gaze following in humans? , 2008, Vision Research.

[9]  G. Butterworth,et al.  How the eyes, head and hand serve definite reference , 2000 .

[10]  A. Goldman,et al.  Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[11]  Rainer Stiefelhagen,et al.  Visual recognition of pointing gestures for human-robot interaction , 2007, Image Vis. Comput..

[12]  H. Bouma,et al.  Perceived orientation of isolated line segments. , 1968, Vision research.

[13]  Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Understanding gesture: is the listener's motor system involved? , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[14]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning , 2004, Nature.

[15]  Werner Deutsch,et al.  The Development of Verbal and Nonverbal Devices for Reference. , 1982 .

[16]  C. L. Schmidt,et al.  Adult Understanding of Spontaneous Attention-Directing Events: What Does Gesture Contribute? , 1999 .

[17]  Norman I. Badler,et al.  Design of a Virtual Human Presenter , 2000, IEEE Computer Graphics and Applications.

[18]  Alexandre Pouget,et al.  Bayesian multisensory integration and cross-modal spatial links , 2004, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[19]  M. Tomasello,et al.  A new look at infant pointing. , 2007, Child development.

[20]  A. D. Salomon Visual field factors in the perception of direction. , 1947, The American journal of psychology.

[21]  Jeff Rickel,et al.  Intelligent Virtual Agents for Education and Training: Opportunities and Challenges , 2001, IVA.

[22]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Parietal Lobe: From Action Organization to Intention Understanding , 2005, Science.

[23]  S. Coren The lateral preference inventory for measurement of handedness, footedness, eyedness, and earedness: Norms for young adults , 1993 .

[24]  I. J. Myung,et al.  Cue Competition Effects: Empirical Tests of Adaptive Network Learning Models , 1993 .

[25]  K. Doya,et al.  A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[26]  M. Pavel,et al.  Extrapolation of linear motion , 1992, Vision Research.

[27]  M. Giese,et al.  Nonvisual Motor Training Influences Biological Motion Perception , 2006, Current Biology.

[28]  V. Gallese,et al.  What is so special about embodied simulation? , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  J. Rieser,et al.  Bayesian integration of spatial information. , 2007, Psychological bulletin.

[30]  A. Bangerter,et al.  Using Pointing and Describing to Achieve Joint Focus of Attention in Dialogue , 2004, Psychological science.

[31]  A. Lacic [A new look]. , 1989, Pielegniarka i polozna.

[32]  G. Butterworth Pointing Is the Royal Road to Language for Babies , 2003 .

[33]  R. Saxe Against simulation: the argument from error , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[34]  D. I. McCloskey,et al.  Pointing , 1988, Behavioural Brain Research.

[35]  Daniel M. Oppenheimer,et al.  Accuracy in detecting referents of pointing gestures unaccompanied by language , 2006 .