Using prediction errors to drive saccade adaptation: the implicit double-step task

A prediction-based error signal, neurally computed as the difference between predicted and observed movement outcomes, has been proposed as the driving force for motor learning. This suggests that the generation of predictive saccades to periodically paced targets—whose performance accuracy is actively maintained using this same error signal—invokes the motor-learning network. We examined whether a simple predictive-saccade task (implicit double-step adaptation, in which targets are gradually displaced outward to exaggerate normal hypometric movement errors) can stand in place of a traditional double-step saccade-adaptation task to induce an increase in saccade gain. We find that the implicit double-step adaptation task can induce significant gain-increase adaptation (of comparable magnitude to that of the standard double-step task) in normal control subjects. Unlike control subjects, patients with impaired cerebella are unable to adapt their saccades in response to this paradigm; this implies that the cerebellum is crucial for processing prediction-based error signals for motor learning.

[1]  Mark Shelhamer,et al.  Saccades exhibit abrupt transition between reactive and predictive; predictive saccade sequences have long-term correlations. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  J. V. Van Gisbergen,et al.  Short-term adaptation of electrically induced saccades in monkey superior colliculus. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  Masaya Hirashima,et al.  Asymmetric Transfer of Visuomotor Learning between Discrete and Rhythmic Movements , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  H. Deubel,et al.  Cerebellar lesions impair rapid saccade amplitude adaptation , 2001, Neurology.

[5]  M. Hallett,et al.  International Cooperative Ataxia Rating Scale for pharmacological assessment of the cerebellar syndrome , 1997, Journal of the Neurological Sciences.

[6]  Aaron L. Wong,et al.  Saccade adaptation improves in response to a gradually introduced stimulus perturbation , 2011, Neuroscience Letters.

[7]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Internal models in the cerebellum , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[8]  R. Leigh,et al.  The neurology of eye movements , 2006 .

[9]  Stefan Schaal,et al.  Forward models in visuomotor control. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  A. Bastian Learning to predict the future: the cerebellum adapts feedforward movement control , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[11]  Mark Shelhamer,et al.  Exploring the Fundamental Dynamics of Error-Based Motor Learning Using a Stationary Predictive-Saccade Task , 2011, PloS one.

[12]  Sarah E. Criscimagna-Hemminger,et al.  Size of error affects cerebellar contributions to motor learning. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  A. G. Lasker,et al.  Cognitive influences on predictive saccadic tracking , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  H. Sawa,et al.  Neuropathological and molecular studies of spinocerebellar ataxia type 6 (SCA6) , 1998, Acta Neuropathologica.

[15]  Mark Shelhamer,et al.  Sensorimotor adaptation error signals are derived from realistic predictions of movement outcomes. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  C. Kennard,et al.  Predictive eye saccades are different from visually triggered saccades , 1987, Vision Research.

[17]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Sensory prediction errors drive cerebellum-dependent adaptation of reaching. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  The control of saccadic adaptation: implications for the scanning of natural visual scenes , 2000, Vision Research.

[19]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Cerebellar Contributions to Adaptive Control of Saccades in Humans , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  D. Robinson,et al.  A METHOD OF MEASURING EYE MOVEMENT USING A SCLERAL SEARCH COIL IN A MAGNETIC FIELD. , 1963, IEEE transactions on bio-medical engineering.

[21]  S. C. Mclaughlin Parametric adjustment in saccadic eye movements , 1967 .

[22]  K. Honjo,et al.  Quantitative assessment of cerebral blood flow in genetically confirmed spinocerebellar ataxia type 6. , 2004, Archives of neurology.

[23]  Wilsaan M. Joiner,et al.  Cerebellar Influence in Oculomotor Phase‐Transition Behavior , 2005, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[24]  D. Henson Corrective saccades: Effects of altering visual feedback , 1978, Vision Research.

[25]  I. Kanazawa,et al.  Clinical, neuropathological, and molecular study in two families with spinocerebellar ataxia type 6 (SCA6) , 1999, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[26]  S. M. Morton,et al.  Cerebellar Contributions to Locomotor Adaptations during Splitbelt Treadmill Walking , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  Thérèse Collins,et al.  The relative importance of retinal error and prediction in saccadic adaptation. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  Sarah E. Criscimagna-Hemminger,et al.  Cerebellar Contributions to Reach Adaptation and Learning Sensory Consequences of Action , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  Mark Shelhamer,et al.  Sequences of Predictive Saccades Are Correlated Over a Span of ∼2 s and Produce a Fractal Time Series , 2005 .

[30]  Masahiko Fujita,et al.  Selective and delay adaptation of human saccades. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[31]  W. Zangemeister,et al.  Disturbance of Predictive Response Initiation of Eye and Head Movements in Cerebellar Patients , 2008, European Neurology.

[32]  R C Miall,et al.  The cerebellum, predictive control and motor coordination. , 2007, Novartis Foundation symposium.

[33]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Spontaneous recovery of motor memory during saccade adaptation. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  Sequences of predictive saccades are correlated over a span of approximately 2 s and produce a fractal time series. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  Lawrence Stark,et al.  Predictive Control of Eye Tracking Movements , 1962 .

[36]  Denis Pélisson,et al.  Long-lasting modifications of saccadic eye movements following adaptation induced in the double-step target paradigm. , 2005, Learning & memory.

[37]  J. Krakauer,et al.  An Implicit Plan Overrides an Explicit Strategy during Visuomotor Adaptation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  W. B. Templeton,et al.  Saccadic plasticity: parametric adaptive control by retinal feedback. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[39]  Predictive Saccades to a Regularly Alternating Target in Cerebellar Patients , 2005, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[40]  P. Thier,et al.  Reduced saccadic resilience and impaired saccadic adaptation due to cerebellar disease , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[41]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control , 1996, Neural Networks.