Open and reproducible science practices in psychoneuroendocrinology: Opportunities to foster scientific progress
暂无分享,去创建一个
T. Stalder | S. Lupien | T. Lonsdorf | S. Laufer | R. Linz | L. Puhlmann | Maria Meier | M. Sicorello | Maurizio Sicorello
[1] K. Roelofs,et al. The STRESS-NL database: A resource for human acute stress studies across the Netherlands , 2022, Psychoneuroendocrinology.
[2] Jade S. Pickering,et al. A community-sourced glossary of open scholarship terms , 2022, Nature Human Behaviour.
[3] Max Kozlov. NIH issues a seismic mandate: share data publicly , 2022, Nature.
[4] E. Henderson. A guide to preregistration and Registered Reports , 2022 .
[5] Christian Merz,et al. Fachgruppe Biologische Psychologie und Neuropsychologie. Mehr als nur fragwürdig: Reproduzierbarkeit und Open Science in der Lehre aus Sicht der Biologischen Psychologie und Neuropsychologie , 2022, Psychologische Rundschau.
[6] S. Lupien,et al. The Cortisol Assessment List (CoAL) A tool to systematically document and evaluate cortisol assessment in blood, urine and saliva , 2021, Comprehensive psychoneuroendocrinology.
[7] D. Quintana. Towards better hypothesis tests in oxytocin research: Evaluating the validity of auxiliary assumptions , 2021, Psychoneuroendocrinology.
[8] Matthew K. Lau,et al. A large-scale study on research code quality and execution , 2021, Scientific Data.
[9] C. Chambers,et al. The past, present and future of Registered Reports , 2021, Nature Human Behaviour.
[10] Dustin A. Fife,et al. Understanding the exploratory/confirmatory data analysis continuum: Moving beyond the "replication crisis". , 2021, The American psychologist.
[11] J. Logg,et al. Pre-registration: Weighing costs and benefits for researchers , 2021 .
[12] Michèle B. Nuijten,et al. Replicability, Robustness, and Reproducibility in Psychological Science. , 2021, Annual review of psychology.
[13] J. Pruessner,et al. Effects of psychological, sensory, and metabolic energy prime manipulation on the acute endocrine stress response in fasted women , 2021, Psychoneuroendocrinology.
[14] Rob Hudson,et al. Explicating Exact versus Conceptual Replication , 2021, Erkenntnis.
[15] A. R. Otto,et al. Acute Psychosocial Stress Increases Cognitive-Effort Avoidance , 2021, Psychological science.
[16] Timothy M. Errington,et al. Initial evidence of research quality of registered reports compared with the standard publishing model , 2021, Nature Human Behaviour.
[17] M. Higgs,et al. Research on registered report research. , 2021, Nature human behaviour.
[18] S. Lupien,et al. Staying informed without a cost: No effect of positive news media on stress reactivity, memory and affect in young adults , 2021, PloS one.
[19] Manuel Kuhn,et al. Navigating the manifold of skin conductance response quantification approaches , 2021 .
[20] P. Sachdev,et al. The need for a reliable oxytocin assay , 2021, Molecular Psychiatry.
[21] A. Keil,et al. Open science in psychophysiology: An overview of challenges and emerging solutions. , 2021, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[22] R. Ebstein,et al. Intranasal oxytocin in the treatment of autism spectrum disorders: A multilevel meta-analysis , 2021, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[23] M. Del Giudice,et al. A Traveler’s Guide to the Multiverse: Promises, Pitfalls, and a Framework for the Evaluation of Analytic Decisions , 2021, Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science.
[24] G. Domes,et al. Effects of glucose intake on stress reactivity in young, healthy men , 2020, Psychoneuroendocrinology.
[25] J. Pruessner,et al. The impact of maternal care and blood glucose availability on the cortisol stress response in fasted women , 2020, Journal of Neural Transmission.
[26] Bethany Lassetter,et al. Testosterone fluctuations in response to a democratic election predict partisan attitudes toward the elected leader , 2020, Psychoneuroendocrinology.
[27] Jordan C. Barone,et al. How to study the menstrual cycle: Practical tools and recommendations , 2020, Psychoneuroendocrinology.
[28] Reinder Vos de Wael,et al. Association between hippocampal structure and serum Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF) in healthy adults: A registered report , 2019, NeuroImage.
[29] Soric. Replicability , 2021, Encyclopedic Dictionary of Archaeology.
[30] Pranjal H. Mehta,et al. Estimating the Associations between Big Five Personality Traits, Testosterone, and Cortisol , 2020, Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology.
[31] Leif D. Nelson,et al. Pre‐registration: Why and How , 2020, Journal of Consumer Psychology.
[32] Cooper A. Smout,et al. Open science saves lives: lessons from the COVID-19 pandemic , 2020, bioRxiv.
[33] A. Holcombe,et al. Where Are the Self-Correcting Mechanisms in Science? , 2020, Review of General Psychology.
[34] Leif D. Nelson,et al. Specification curve analysis , 2020, Nature Human Behaviour.
[35] Jenna A. Harder,et al. The Multiverse of Methods: Extending the Multiverse Analysis to Address Data-Collection Decisions , 2020, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.
[36] M. Gollwitzer. DFG Priority Program SPP 2317 Proposal: A meta-scientific program to analyze and optimize replicability in the behavioral, social, and cognitive sciences (META-REP) , 2020 .
[37] Michèle B. Nuijten,et al. “statcheck”: Automatically detect statistical reporting inconsistencies to increase reproducibility of meta‐analyses , 2020, Research synthesis methods.
[38] Daniel S Quintana,et al. A synthetic dataset primer for the biobehavioural sciences to promote reproducibility and hypothesis generation , 2020, eLife.
[39] U. Dirnagl. Preregistration of exploratory research: Learning from the golden age of discovery , 2020, PLoS biology.
[40] C. Chambers,et al. The past, present and future of Registered Reports , 2020, Nature Human Behaviour.
[41] Anne M. Scheel,et al. An Excess of Positive Results: Comparing the Standard Psychology Literature With Registered Reports , 2020, Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science.
[42] Brian A. Nosek,et al. Ensuring the quality and specificity of preregistrations , 2018, PLoS biology.
[43] D. Mehler,et al. Correction: Open science challenges, benefits and tips in early career and beyond , 2019, PLoS biology.
[44] Bradley C. Love,et al. Variability in the analysis of a single neuroimaging dataset by many teams , 2019, Nature.
[45] Jan Richter,et al. Navigating the garden of forking paths for data exclusions in fear conditioning research , 2019, eLife.
[46] Jason M. Scimeca,et al. Practical Considerations for Navigating Registered Reports , 2019, Trends in Neurosciences.
[47] Matthew D. Lieberman,et al. Null results of oxytocin and vasopressin administration across a range of social cognitive and behavioral paradigms: Evidence from a randomized controlled trial , 2019, Psychoneuroendocrinology.
[48] Stuart J. Ritchie,et al. Preregistration of secondary data analysis: A template and tutorial , 2019, Meta-Psychology.
[49] M. Fullana,et al. Fear Extinction Retention: Is It What We Think It Is? , 2019, Biological Psychiatry.
[50] Deborah E. Rupp,et al. Answers to 18 Questions About Open Science Practices , 2019 .
[51] Nicholas A. Coles,et al. Analysis of Open Data and Computational Reproducibility in Registered Reports in Psychology , 2019, Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science.
[52] Gabriel Popkin,et al. Data sharing and how it can benefit your scientific career , 2019, Nature.
[53] Amanda Kvarven,et al. Comparing Meta-Analyses and Pre-Registered Multiple Labs Replication Projects , 2019 .
[54] Herman Aguinis,et al. HARKing: How Badly Can Cherry-Picking and Question Trolling Produce Bias in Published Results? , 2019 .
[55] Russell A. Poldrack,et al. The Costs of Reproducibility , 2019, Neuron.
[56] Mathew H. Evans,et al. Many Analysts, One Data Set: Making Transparent How Variations in Analytic Choices Affect Results , 2018, Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science.
[57] Michael C. Frank,et al. A practical guide for transparency in psychological science , 2018 .
[58] Brian A. Nosek,et al. The preregistration revolution , 2018, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[59] Lei Feng,et al. Systematic review and meta‐analysis of reported adverse events of long‐term intranasal oxytocin treatment for autism spectrum disorder , 2018, Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences.
[60] Michelle N. Meyer,et al. Practical Tips for Ethical Data Sharing , 2018 .
[61] Eric-Jan Wagenmakers,et al. Data Sharing in Psychology: A Survey on Barriers and Preconditions , 2018 .
[62] K. Adolph,et al. Practical Solutions for Sharing Data and Materials From Psychological Research , 2018, Advances in methods and practices in psychological science.
[63] Mark Rubin,et al. When Does HARKing Hurt? Identifying When Different Types of Undisclosed Post Hoc Hypothesizing Harm Scientific Progress , 2017 .
[64] S. Segerstrom,et al. Psychosocial functioning and the cortisol awakening response: Meta-analysis, P-curve analysis, and evaluation of the evidential value in existing studies , 2017, Biological Psychology.
[65] Brian A. Nosek,et al. Evaluating Registered Reports: A Naturalistic Comparative Study of Article Impact , 2017 .
[66] James A. J. Heathers,et al. The GRIM Test , 2017 .
[67] Brian A. Nosek,et al. Making sense of replications , 2017, eLife.
[68] John P. A. Ioannidis,et al. A manifesto for reproducible science , 2017, Nature Human Behaviour.
[69] Eric Dutilh. Seven Selfish Reasons for Preregistration , 2016 .
[70] Gillian M. Raab,et al. synthpop: Bespoke Creation of Synthetic Data in R , 2016 .
[71] Francis Tuerlinckx,et al. Increasing Transparency Through a Multiverse Analysis , 2016, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.
[72] Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al. Why Preregistration Makes Me Nervous , 2016 .
[73] M. Munafo,et al. Null is beautiful: On the importance of publishing null results , 2016, Journal of psychopharmacology.
[74] Satrajit S. Ghosh,et al. The brain imaging data structure, a format for organizing and describing outputs of neuroimaging experiments , 2016, Scientific Data.
[75] Richard McElreath,et al. The natural selection of bad science , 2016, Royal Society Open Science.
[76] S. Goodman,et al. Statistical tests, P values, confidence intervals, and power: a guide to misinterpretations , 2016, European Journal of Epidemiology.
[77] G. Nave,et al. Is there a Publication Bias in Behavioural Intranasal Oxytocin Research on Humans? Opening the File Drawer of One Laboratory , 2016, Journal of neuroendocrinology.
[78] Erik Schultes,et al. The FAIR Guiding Principles for scientific data management and stewardship , 2016, Scientific Data.
[79] E. Adam,et al. Assessment of the cortisol awakening response: Expert consensus guidelines , 2016, Psychoneuroendocrinology.
[80] Colin Camerer,et al. Does Oxytocin Increase Trust in Humans? A Critical Review of Research , 2015, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.
[81] Wilhelm Gaus,et al. Interpretation of Statistical Significance - Exploratory Versus Confirmative Testing in Clinical Trials, Epidemiological Studies, Meta-Analyses and Toxicological Screening (Using Ginkgo biloba as an Example) , 2015 .
[82] I. Cockburn,et al. The Economics of Reproducibility in Preclinical Research , 2015, PLoS biology.
[83] R. Lanfear,et al. The Extent and Consequences of P-Hacking in Science , 2015, PLoS biology.
[84] Andrew H. Miller,et al. The role of inflammation in depression: from evolutionary imperative to modern treatment target , 2015, Nature Reviews Immunology.
[85] Adam R Ferguson,et al. Big data from small data: data-sharing in the 'long tail' of neuroscience , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.
[86] Neil Malhotra,et al. Publication bias in the social sciences: Unlocking the file drawer , 2014, Science.
[87] J. Brian,et al. Intranasal oxytocin in the treatment of autism spectrum disorders: A review of literature and early safety and efficacy data in youth , 2014, Brain Research.
[88] Leif D. Nelson,et al. P-Curve and Effect Size: Correcting for Publication Bias Using Only Significant Results , 2014 .
[89] Leif D. Nelson,et al. p-Curve and Effect Size , 2014, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.
[90] Brian A. Nosek,et al. Registered Reports A Method to Increase the Credibility of Published Results , 2014 .
[91] Leif D. Nelson,et al. P-Curve: A Key to the File Drawer , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[92] A. El-Menyar,et al. Registration of Clinical Trials: Is it Really Needed? , 2013, North American journal of medical sciences.
[93] F. Plessow,et al. Classification Criteria for Distinguishing Cortisol Responders From Nonresponders to Psychosocial Stress: Evaluation of Salivary Cortisol Pulse Detection in Panel Designs , 2013, Psychosomatic medicine.
[94] F. Plessow,et al. Transformation techniques for cross-sectional and longitudinal endocrine data: Application to salivary cortisol concentrations , 2013, Psychoneuroendocrinology.
[95] C. Chambers. Registered Reports: A new publishing initiative at Cortex , 2013, Cortex.
[96] Brian A. Nosek,et al. Scientific Utopia , 2012, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.
[97] G. Loewenstein,et al. Measuring the Prevalence of Questionable Research Practices With Incentives for Truth Telling , 2012, Psychological science.
[98] O. Wolkowitz,et al. Maintenance of a positive outlook during acute stress protects against pro-inflammatory reactivity and future depressive symptoms , 2012, Brain, Behavior, and Immunity.
[99] Leif D. Nelson,et al. False-Positive Psychology , 2011, Psychological science.
[100] Daniele Fanelli,et al. Negative results are disappearing from most disciplines and countries , 2011, Scientometrics.
[101] R. Brand,et al. Testing for the presence of positive-outcome bias in peer review: a randomized controlled trial. , 2010, Archives of internal medicine.
[102] E. Maywood,et al. Circadian clocks: regulators of endocrine and metabolic rhythms. , 2007, The Journal of endocrinology.
[103] Markus Schwaninger,et al. A mechanism converting psychosocial stress into mononuclear cell activation , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[104] D. Rennie. CONSORT revised--improving the reporting of randomized trials. , 2001, JAMA.
[105] Gerry White,et al. The Past , 2000 .
[106] N. Kerr. HARKing: Hypothesizing After the Results are Known , 1998, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.
[107] C. Kirschbaum,et al. The 'Trier Social Stress Test'--a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. , 1993, Neuropsychobiology.
[108] R. Rosenthal. The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results , 1979 .
[109] K. Pearson,et al. Statistical Tests , 1935, Nature.