The Development of Children's Problem Solving in a Gears Task: A Problem Space Perspective

I examined two contexts of development in children's problem solving: (1) the macro-context of different age cohorts (8–9 vs 11–12 years of age); and (2) the micro-context of an approximately one-hour experimental session. Twenty subjects (even divided across sexes and these two age groups) were individually presented a collection of variously sized gears, a board onto which these gears could be easily attached and rotated, and a knob. Each subject was asked to find all solutions, in which two marked gears were turning the same way, and to represent these solutions graphically. Subjects applied four different problem spaces to the task: the Euclidean, the Kinematic, the Dynamic, and the Topological. The Arithmetic Modifier could be applied to any of these problem spaces, resulting in a numerical characterization of the gear constructions and/or production strategies. The 11–12's tended to shift problem spaces adaptively; the 8–9's seldom did so. Analysis of the pathways of transition between the problem spaces revealed a complex picture of partial or complete incorporation, and substitution.

[1]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. , 1977 .

[2]  H. Simon,et al.  The functional equivalence of problem solving skills , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[3]  H. Montgomery,et al.  Knowledge and Technique in Statistical Problem‐Solving , 1981 .

[4]  F. J. Langdon,et al.  The Child's Conception of Space , 1967 .

[5]  H. Montgomery,et al.  Detection of errors in statistical problem solving , 1982 .

[6]  H A Simon,et al.  The theory of learning by doing. , 1979, Psychological review.

[7]  J. Piaget,et al.  The Growth Of Logical Thinking From Childhood To Adolescence: An Essay On The Construction Of Formal Operational Structures , 1958 .

[8]  Allen Newell,et al.  Human Problem Solving. , 1973 .

[9]  Monique Laurendeau,et al.  The development of the concept of space in the child , 1970 .

[10]  A. Silverstein The Developmental Psychology of Jean Piaget , 1965 .

[11]  B. Inhelder,et al.  If you want to get ahead, get a theory , 1975, Cognition.

[12]  Herbert A. Simon,et al.  Understanding written problem instructions. , 1974 .

[13]  John McCarthy,et al.  SOME PHILOSOPHICAL PROBLEMS FROM THE STANDPOINT OF ARTI CIAL INTELLIGENCE , 1987 .

[14]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  The Effects of Simple Necessity and Sufficiency Relationships on Children's Causal Inferences. , 1976 .

[15]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Verbal reports as data. , 1980 .

[16]  K. Duncker,et al.  On problem-solving , 1945 .

[17]  A. Millington Problem Solving in Physics , 1968, Nature.

[18]  H. Eysenck THINKING , 1958 .

[19]  D. Klahr,et al.  Formal assessment of problem-solving and planning processes in preschool children , 1981, Cognitive Psychology.

[20]  R. Siegler Developmental Sequences within and between Concepts. , 1981 .

[21]  Herbert A. Simon,et al.  Problem solving and rule induction: A unified view. , 1974 .

[22]  Gideon Keren,et al.  On the importance of identifying the correct ‘problem space’ , 1984, Cognition.

[23]  J. Piaget,et al.  The Development of Thought: Equilibration of Cognitive Structures , 1977 .

[24]  José Marques Des structures cognitives aux representations sociales , 1983 .

[25]  J. A. Landeweerd,et al.  Internal representation of a process, fault diagnosis and fault correction , 1979 .