Dissociating Early and Late Error Signals in Perceptual Recognition

Decisions about object identity follow a period in which evidence is gathered and analyzed. Evidence can consist of both task-relevant external stimuli and internally generated goals and expectations. How the various pieces of information are gathered and filtered into meaningful evidence by the nervous system is largely unknown. Although object recognition is often highly efficient and accurate, errors are common. Errors may be related to faulty evidence gathering arising from early misinterpretations of incoming stimulus information. In addition, errors in task performance are known to elicit late corrective performance monitoring mechanisms that can optimize or otherwise adjust future behavior. In this study, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in an extended trial paradigm of object recognition to study whether we could identify performance-based signal modulations prior to and following the moment of recognition. The rationale driving the current report is that early modulations in fMRI activity may reflect faulty evidence gathering, whereas late modulations may reflect the presence of performance monitoring mechanisms. We tested this possibility by comparing fMRI activity on correct and error trials in regions of interest (ROIs) that were selected a priori. We found pre- and postrecognition accuracy-dependent modulation in different sets of a priori ROIs, suggesting the presence of dissociable error signals.

[1]  C Frith,et al.  Brain mechanisms associated with top-down processes in perception. , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[2]  J. Fuster The Prefrontal Cortex , 1997 .

[3]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Separating Processes within a Trial in Event-Related Functional MRI I. The Method , 2001, NeuroImage.

[4]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Why is there an ERN/Ne on correct trials? Response representations, stimulus-related components, and the theory of error-processing , 2001, Biological Psychology.

[5]  Jack L. Lancaster,et al.  A modality‐independent approach to spatial normalization of tomographic images of the human brain , 1995 .

[6]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The Neural Basis of Economic Decision-Making in the Ultimatum Game , 2003, Science.

[7]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Optimal decision making and the anterior cingulate cortex , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[8]  S. Petersen,et al.  Characterizing the Hemodynamic Response: Effects of Presentation Rate, Sampling Procedure, and the Possibility of Ordering Brain Activity Based on Relative Timing , 2000, NeuroImage.

[9]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[10]  J. Schall,et al.  Neural Control of Voluntary Movement Initiation , 1996, Science.

[11]  Elisabeth J. Ploran,et al.  Evidence Accumulation and the Moment of Recognition: Dissociating Perceptual Recognition Processes Using fMRI , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  D. Vickers,et al.  Evidence for an accumulator model of psychophysical discrimination. , 1970, Ergonomics.

[13]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model. , 2001, Psychological review.

[15]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Analysis of functional MRI time‐series , 1994, Human Brain Mapping.

[16]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[17]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Separating Processes within a Trial in Event-Related Functional MRI II. Analysis , 2001, NeuroImage.

[18]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  Dual diffusion model for single-cell recording data from the superior colliculus in a brightness-discrimination task. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory , 1995, Nature.

[20]  M. Posner,et al.  Localization of a Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1994 .

[21]  M. Raichle,et al.  Anatomic Localization and Quantitative Analysis of Gradient Refocused Echo-Planar fMRI Susceptibility Artifacts , 1997, NeuroImage.

[22]  Matthew Flatt,et al.  PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers , 1993 .

[23]  A. Parker,et al.  Sense and the single neuron: probing the physiology of perception. , 1998, Annual review of neuroscience.

[24]  E. Salas,et al.  Taking stock of naturalistic decision making , 2001 .

[25]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Neural correlates of a decision in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the macaque , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[26]  M. Coles,et al.  Performance monitoring in a confusing world: error-related brain activity, judgments of response accuracy, and types of errors. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  L. Pessoa,et al.  Neural Correlates of Perceptual Choice and Decision Making during Fear–Disgust Discrimination , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  R. Audley,et al.  SOME ALTERNATIVE STOCHASTIC MODELS OF CHOICE1 , 1965 .

[29]  T. Robbins,et al.  Choosing between Small, Likely Rewards and Large, Unlikely Rewards Activates Inferior and Orbital Prefrontal Cortex , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. , 1998, Science.

[31]  R. Myers Quantification of brain function using PET , 1996 .

[32]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  The effects of visual object priming on brain activation before and after recognition , 2000, Current Biology.

[33]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[34]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  A comparison of macaque behavior and superior colliculus neuronal activity to predictions from models of two-choice decisions. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  B. Rossion,et al.  Revisiting Snodgrass and Vanderwart's Object Pictorial Set: The Role of Surface Detail in Basic-Level Object Recognition , 2004, Perception.

[36]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Selective and divided attention during visual discriminations of shape, color, and speed: functional anatomy by positron emission tomography , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[37]  M. Bar A Cortical Mechanism for Triggering Top-Down Facilitation in Visual Object Recognition , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[38]  Robert T. Knight,et al.  Top-down Enhancement and Suppression of the Magnitude and Speed of Neural Activity , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[39]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. , 2004, Psychological review.

[40]  M. Posner,et al.  Localization of cognitive operations in the human brain. , 1988, Science.

[41]  Mark E. Wheeler,et al.  Left posterior parietal cortex participates in both task preparation and episodic retrieval , 2009, NeuroImage.

[42]  W. Newsome,et al.  Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  Michael L. Platt,et al.  Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex , 1999, Nature.

[44]  H. Spinnler The prefrontal cortex, Anatomy, physiology, and neuropsychology of the frontal lobe, J.M. Fuster. Raven Press, New York (1980), IX-222 pages , 1981 .

[45]  L. Fellows The cognitive neuroscience of human decision making: a review and conceptual framework. , 2004, Behavioral and cognitive neuroscience reviews.

[46]  T. Braver,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[47]  A. Engel,et al.  Trial-by-Trial Coupling of Concurrent Electroencephalogram and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Identifies the Dynamics of Performance Monitoring , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  J. Gold,et al.  Representation of a perceptual decision in developing oculomotor commands , 2000, Nature.

[49]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.

[50]  I. THE ATTENTION SYSTEM OF THE HUMAN BRAIN , 2002 .

[51]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Dynamics of visual recognition revealed by fMRI , 2006, NeuroImage.

[52]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  ERP components on reaction errors and their functional significance: a tutorial , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[53]  David L. Sheinberg,et al.  Visual object recognition. , 1996, Annual review of neuroscience.

[54]  Gordon D Logan,et al.  An instance theory of attention and memory. , 2002, Psychological review.

[55]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Dynamics of neuronal responses in macaque MT and VIP during motion detection , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[56]  S. Rauch,et al.  The counting stroop: An interference task specialized for functional neuroimaging—validation study with functional MRI , 1998, Human brain mapping.

[57]  J. Gold,et al.  The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[58]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Neural correlates of incongruous visual information An event-related fMRI study , 2003, NeuroImage.

[59]  Jeffrey S. Johnson,et al.  The recognition of partially visible natural objects in the presence and absence of their occluders , 2005, Vision Research.

[60]  M. Posner,et al.  Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.

[61]  Kristina M. Visscher,et al.  A Core System for the Implementation of Task Sets , 2006, Neuron.

[62]  G. Glover,et al.  Error‐related brain activation during a Go/NoGo response inhibition task , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[63]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  The accumulator model of two-choice discrimination , 1988 .

[64]  Jennifer L. Ferris,et al.  Lateral inferior prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex are engaged at different stages in the solution of insight problems , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[65]  J. Hirsch,et al.  A Neural Representation of Categorization Uncertainty in the Human Brain , 2006, Neuron.

[66]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Response of Neurons in the Lateral Intraparietal Area during a Combined Visual Discrimination Reaction Time Task , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[67]  B. J. Winer Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 1992 .

[68]  David I. Donaldson,et al.  Remember the Source: Dissociating Frontal and Parietal Contributions to Episodic Memory , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[69]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Learned Predictions of Error Likelihood in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2005, Science.

[70]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .

[71]  Emilio Salinas,et al.  Cognitive neuroscience: Flutter Discrimination: neural codes, perception, memory and decision making , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[72]  S. Link,et al.  A sequential theory of psychological discrimination , 1975 .

[73]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Cerebral Cortex doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj097 Role of Prefrontal and Anterior Cingulate Regions in Decision-Making Processes Shared by Memory and Nonmemory Tasks , 2005 .

[74]  P. Strick,et al.  Motor areas of the medial wall: a review of their location and functional activation. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[75]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature.

[76]  C. Olson,et al.  Functional heterogeneity in cingulate cortex: the anterior executive and posterior evaluative regions. , 1992, Cerebral cortex.

[77]  Abraham Z. Snyder,et al.  Transient BOLD responses at block transitions , 2005, NeuroImage.

[78]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Decoding the neural substrates of reward-related decision making with functional MRI , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.