Numeracy and Interpretation of Prognostic Estimates in Intracerebral Hemorrhage Among Surrogate Decision Makers in the Neurologic ICU

Objective: Clinicians caring for patients with intracerebral hemorrhage must often discuss prognosis and goals of care with their patients’ surrogate decision makers, and may make numeric estimates of likelihood of survival and functional independence, informed by validated prediction models. Surrogates’ prognostic estimates are often discordant with physicians’, suggesting that physicians’ numeric statements may not be accurately interpreted. We sought to assess the relationship between numeracy and interpretation of prognostic estimates in intracerebral hemorrhage among surrogate decision makers. We also assessed surrogates’ application of prognostic estimates to decisions regarding goals of care. Design: Single-center, survey-based, cross-sectional study. Setting: Twenty-two–bed neurologic ICU at an urban, academic hospital. Subjects: Surrogate decision makers for patients admitted to the neurologic ICU. Interventions: Participants completed a survey containing five clinical vignettes describing patients with nontraumatic intracerebral hemorrhage. For each patient, numerical estimates of survival and functional independence were explicitly provided, based on the validated outcome risk stratification scale (intracerebral hemorrhage score) and the Prediction of Functional Outcome in Patients with Primary Intracerebral Hemorrhage score. Measurements and Main Results: Participants were asked to make their own prognostic estimates, as well as to describe their preferred goals of care for each hypothetical patient. Respondent demographics were collected, and numeracy was assessed using a modified Lipkus 11-item scale. Poor numeracy was common (42 of 96 total subjects) in this relatively highly educated population. Most prognostic estimates (55%) made by surrogates were discordant with the provided estimates. High numeracy correlated with better concordance (odds ratio, 23.9 [5.57–97.64]; p < 0.001), independent of several factors, including level of education and religion. Numeracy also affected goals-of-care decisions made by surrogates. Conclusions: Poor numeracy is common among surrogate decision makers in an intensive care setting and poses a barrier to communication between surrogates and clinicians regarding prognosis and goals of care.

[1]  Margaret L. Schwarze,et al.  Managing Uncertainty - Harnessing the Power of Scenario Planning. , 2017, The New England journal of medicine.

[2]  M. D. Hashem,et al.  Evaluation of a strategy for enrolling the families of critically ill patients in research using limited human resources , 2017, PloS one.

[3]  D. Hanley,et al.  Intensive Blood-Pressure Lowering in Patients with Acute Cerebral Hemorrhage. , 2016, The New England journal of medicine.

[4]  L. Weissfeld,et al.  Prevalence of and Factors Related to Discordance About Prognosis Between Physicians and Surrogate Decision Makers of Critically Ill Patients. , 2016, JAMA.

[5]  J. Michalak,et al.  Impaired intuition in patients with major depressive disorder. , 2015, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[6]  R. Arnold,et al.  Quality of Communication in the ICU and Surrogate’s Understanding of Prognosis* , 2015, Critical care medicine.

[7]  A. Verceles,et al.  Half of the family members of critically ill patients experience excessive daytime sleepiness , 2014, Intensive Care Medicine.

[8]  D. Wendler,et al.  Use of a patient preference predictor to help make medical decisions for incapacitated patients. , 2014, The Journal of medicine and philosophy.

[9]  G. Netzer,et al.  Recognizing, naming, and measuring a family intensive care unit syndrome. , 2014, Annals of the American Thoracic Society.

[10]  Alexandra Suppes,et al.  Surrogate expectations in severe brain injury , 2013, Brain injury.

[11]  D. Sulmasy,et al.  Spontaneous intracerebral hemorrhage and the challenge of surgical decision making: a review. , 2013, Neurosurgical focus.

[12]  D. White,et al.  Surrogate Decision Makers' Interpretation of Prognostic Information , 2012, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[13]  D. White,et al.  A randomized trial of two methods to disclose prognosis to surrogate decision makers in intensive care units. , 2010, American journal of respiratory and critical care medicine.

[14]  B. Lo,et al.  “It’s not just what the doctor tells me:” Factors that influence surrogate decision-makers’ perceptions of prognosis* , 2010, Critical care medicine.

[15]  M. Galesic,et al.  Statistical numeracy for health: a cross-cultural comparison with probabilistic national samples. , 2010, Archives of internal medicine.

[16]  V. Reyna,et al.  How numeracy influences risk comprehension and medical decision making. , 2009, Psychological bulletin.

[17]  Victor M Montori,et al.  Perspective: The Role of Numeracy in Health Care , 2008, Journal of health communication.

[18]  Angela Fagerlin,et al.  Clinical Implications of Numeracy: Theory and Practice , 2008, Annals of behavioral medicine : a publication of the Society of Behavioral Medicine.

[19]  Eric E. Smith,et al.  Prediction of Functional Outcome in Patients With Primary Intracerebral Hemorrhage: The FUNC Score , 2008, Stroke.

[20]  Anne K Chipman,et al.  Doubt and belief in physicians’ ability to prognosticate during critical illness: The perspective of surrogate decision makers , 2008, Critical care medicine.

[21]  Susan Persky,et al.  Assessing hypothetical scenario methodology in genetic susceptibility testing analog studies: a quantitative review , 2007, Genetics in Medicine.

[22]  P. Polo-Kantola,et al.  Sleep deprivation: Impact on cognitive performance , 2007, Neuropsychiatric disease and treatment.

[23]  L. Lisabeth,et al.  Early care limitations independently predict mortality after intracerebral hemorrhage , 2007, Neurology.

[24]  B. Lo,et al.  Prognostication during physician-family discussions about limiting life support in intensive care units* , 2007, Critical care medicine.

[25]  Elizabeth Dugan,et al.  Development of abbreviated measures to assess patient trust in a physician, a health insurer, and the medical profession , 2005, BMC health services research.

[26]  Steven Woloshin,et al.  Can Patients Interpret Health Information? An Assessment of the Medical Data Interpretation Test , 2005, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[27]  É. Azoulay,et al.  Symptoms of anxiety and depression in family members of intensive care unit patients before discharge or death. A prospective multicenter study. , 2005, Journal of critical care.

[28]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  Projection Bias in Medical Decision Making , 2005, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[29]  D. Patrick,et al.  Patient-physician communication about end-of-life care for patients with severe COPD , 2004, European Respiratory Journal.

[30]  R. Spitzer,et al.  The Patient Health Questionnaire-2: Validity of a Two-Item Depression Screener , 2003, Medical care.

[31]  D. Uehlinger,et al.  Medical futility: Predicting outcome of intensive care unit patients by nurses and doctors—A prospective comparative study* , 2003, Critical care medicine.

[32]  G. Manley,et al.  The ICH Score , 2001 .

[33]  H. Winn,et al.  Withdrawal of support in intracerebral hemorrhage may lead to self-fulfilling prophecies , 2001, Neurology.

[34]  B. Rimer,et al.  General Performance on a Numeracy Scale among Highly Educated Samples , 2001, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[35]  N. Christakis,et al.  Extent and determinants of error in doctors' prognoses in terminally ill patients: prospective cohort study , 2000, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[36]  J. De Keyser,et al.  Use of the Barthel index and modified Rankin scale in acute stroke trials. , 1999, Stroke.

[37]  Lisa M. Schwartz,et al.  The Role of Numeracy in Understanding the Benefit of Screening Mammography , 1997, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[38]  J. Woo,et al.  Dichotomizing stroke outcomes based on self-reported dependency , 1997, Neurology.

[39]  T Brott,et al.  The ABCs of measuring intracerebral hemorrhage volumes. , 1996, Stroke.

[40]  G Gigerenzer,et al.  The Psychology of Good Judgment , 1996, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[41]  S Greenland,et al.  Modeling and variable selection in epidemiologic analysis. , 1989, American journal of public health.

[42]  J. Cuzick,et al.  A Wilcoxon-type test for trend. , 1985, Statistics in medicine.

[43]  B. Lo,et al.  Surrogate decision-makers' perspectives on discussing prognosis in the face of uncertainty. , 2009, American journal of respiratory and critical care medicine.

[44]  Lisa M. Schwartz,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE IN THE PUBLIC INTEREST Helping Doctors and Patients Make Sense of Health Statistics , 2022 .