Incompletely Informed Shorebirds That Face a Digestive Constraint Maximize Net Energy Gain When Exploiting Patches
暂无分享,去创建一个
Theunis Piersma | J. V. van Gils | T. Piersma | Jan A. van Gils | Ingrid W. Schenk | Oscar Bos | I. W. Schenk | O. Bos
[1] G. Visser,et al. Cost-benefit analysis of mollusc eating in a shorebird I. Foraging and processing costs estimated by the doubly labelled water method , 2003, Journal of Experimental Biology.
[2] A. Kacelnik,et al. Optimal foraging and arbitrary food distributions: Patch models gain a lease of life , 1988 .
[3] C. S. Holling. Some Characteristics of Simple Types of Predation and Parasitism , 1959, The Canadian Entomologist.
[4] B. Nolet. Efficiency as a foraging currency in animals attaining a gain below the energetic ceiling , 2002 .
[5] Richard F. Green,et al. Stochastic Models of Optimal Foraging , 1987 .
[6] Jonathan M. Jeschke,et al. PREDATOR FUNCTIONAL RESPONSES: DISCRIMINATING BETWEEN HANDLING AND DIGESTING PREY , 2002 .
[7] Theunis Piersma,et al. Scale and intensity of intertidal habitat use by knots Calidris canutus in the Western Wadden Sea in relation to food, friends and foes , 1993 .
[8] J. McNamara. Optimal patch use in a stochastic environment , 1982 .
[9] M. Rodríguez-Gironés,et al. Density-Dependent Patch Exploitation and Acquisition of Environmental Information , 1997, Theoretical population biology.
[10] Joel s. Brown,et al. Patch use as an indicator of habitat preference, predation risk, and competition , 2004, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[11] Ola Olsson,et al. The survival-rate-maximizing policy for Bayesian foragers: wait for good news , 1998 .
[12] L. Zwarts,et al. Annual and seasonal-variation in the food-supply harvestable by knot Calidris-canutus staging in the Wadden Sea in late summer , 1992 .
[13] S. Daan,et al. Seasonality in basal metabolic rate and thermal conductance in a long-distance migrant shorebird, the knot (Calidris canutus) , 1995, Journal of Comparative Physiology B.
[14] Samuel Karlin,et al. A First Course on Stochastic Processes , 1968 .
[15] P. Chesson,et al. Foraging in a patchy environment: prey-encounter rate and residence time distributions , 1989, Animal Behaviour.
[16] Larry B. Crowder,et al. Bluegills continuously update patch giving-up times based on foraging experience , 1994, Animal Behaviour.
[17] James N. McNair,et al. Optimal Giving-Up Times and the Marginal Value Theorem , 1982, The American Naturalist.
[18] T. Piersma,et al. An evaluation of intertidal feeding habitats from a shorebird perspective: Towards relevant comparisons between temperate and tropical mudflats , 1993 .
[19] Ola Olsson,et al. A Three-Neuron Model of Information Processing During Bayesian Foraging , 2000, ANNIMAB.
[20] R. Ydenberg,et al. Simple models of feeding with time and enery contstraints , 1998 .
[21] Joel s. Brown,et al. Patch Assessment in Fox Squirrels: The Role of Resource Density, Patch Size, and Patch Boundaries , 1996, The American Naturalist.
[22] O. Olsson,et al. Optimal Bayesian foraging policies and prey population dynamics-some comments on Rodriguez-Girones and Vasquez. , 2000, Theoretical population biology.
[23] A. Kacelnik,et al. Optimal Foraging and Beyond: How Starlings Cope with Changes in Food Availability , 1998, The American Naturalist.
[24] G. Chelazzi,et al. When time is of the essence: choosing a currency for prey-handling costs , 2000 .
[25] A. Hedenström,et al. OPTIMAL FLIGHT SPEED OF BIRDS , 1995 .
[26] Thomas J. Valone,et al. Bayesian and prescient assessment: foraging with pre-harvest information , 1991, Animal Behaviour.
[27] L. Zwarts,et al. Why knot Calidris canutus take medium-sized Macoma balthica when six prey species are available , 1992 .
[28] Flavia Chiarotti,et al. Statistical analysis of behavioral data. , 2005, Current protocols in toxicology.
[29] Bernard Dacorogna,et al. Optimal Foraging on Arbitrary Food Distributions and the Definition of Habitat Patches , 1988, The American Naturalist.
[30] Richard F. Green,et al. Bayesian birds: A simple example of Oaten's stochastic model of optimal foraging , 1980 .
[31] Roberts Wm,et al. The Problem of Temporal Scale in Optimization: Three Contrasting Views of Hummingbird Visits to Flowers , 1992 .
[32] P. Haccou,et al. The Influence of Larval Dispersal in the Cinnabar Moth (Tyria jacobaeae) on Predation by the Red Wood Ant (Formica polyctena): An Analysis Based on the Proportional Hazards Model , 1985 .
[33] Theunis Piersma,et al. Effects of Microhabitat, Flocking, Climate and Migratory Goal on Energy Expenditure in the Annual Cycle of Red Knots , 1994 .
[34] Leo R. M. Maas,et al. A new pressure sensory mechanism for prey detection in birds: the use of principles of seabed dynamics? , 1998, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[35] A I Houston,et al. Currencies For Foraging Based on Energetic Gain , 1997, The American Naturalist.
[36] N. B. Kotliar,et al. Multiple scales of patchiness and patch structure: a hierarchical framework for the study of heterogeneity , 1990 .
[37] C. Clark,et al. Dynamic State Variable Models in Ecology , 2000 .
[38] S. Fretwell,et al. On territorial behavior and other factors influencing habitat distribution in birds , 1969 .
[39] S R Hursh,et al. Economic concepts for the analysis of behavior. , 1980, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[40] A. Kacelnik,et al. To walk or to fly? How birds choose among foraging modes. , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[41] A. Oaten,et al. Optimal foraging in patches: a case for stochasticity. , 1977, Theoretical population biology.
[42] T. Piersma. Energetic Bottlenecks and Other Design Constraints in Avian Annual Cycles1 , 2002, Integrative and comparative biology.
[43] Ola Olsson,et al. Gaining ecological information about Bayesian foragers through their behaviour. II. A field test with woodpeckers , 1999 .
[44] C. L. Gass,et al. The Problem of Temporal Scale in Optimization: Three Contrasting Views of Hummingbird Visits to Flowers , 1992, The American Naturalist.
[45] S. Fretwell,et al. On territorial behavior and other factors influencing habitat distribution in birds , 1969 .
[46] J. V. van Gils,et al. Cost-benefit analysis of mollusc-eating in a shorebird II. Optimizing gizzard size in the face of seasonal demands , 2003, Journal of Experimental Biology.
[47] J. Gils,et al. Holling's functional response model as a tool to link the food-finding mechanism of a probing shorebird with its spatial distribution , 1995 .
[48] J. Krebs,et al. Hunting by expectation or optimal foraging: A study of patch use by chickadees , 1974 .
[49] A. Houston. Energetic constraints and foraging efficiency , 1995 .
[50] E. Charnov. Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem. , 1976, Theoretical population biology.
[51] Theunis Piersma,et al. Carrying large fuel loads during sustained bird flight is cheaper than expected , 2001, Nature.
[52] S. L. Lima,et al. Downy Woodpecker Foraging Behavior: Efficient Sampling in Simple Stochastic Environments , 1984 .
[53] T. Piersma,et al. Resources for long-distance migration of knots Calidris canutus islandica and C. c. canutus : How broad is the temporal exploitation window of benthic prey in the western and eastern Wadden Sea? , 1994 .
[54] Ola Olsson,et al. Gain curves in depletable food patches: A test of five models with European starlings , 2001 .
[55] Theunis Piersma,et al. Close to the edge: Energetic bottlenecks and the evolution of migratory pathways in knots , 1994 .
[56] Thomas J. Valone,et al. Measuring Patch Assessment Abilities of Desert Granivores , 1989 .
[57] Y. Iwasa,et al. Prey Distribution as a Factor Determining the Choice of Optimal Foraging Strategy , 1981, The American Naturalist.