Getting to the Meaning of the Regular Past Tense: Evidence from Neuropsychology

Neuropsychological impairments of English past tense processing inform a key debate in cognitive neuroscience concerning the nature of mental mechanisms. Dual-route accounts claim that regular past tense comprehension deficits reflect a specific impairment of morphological decomposition (e.g., jump + ed), disrupting the automatic comprehension of word meaning accessed via the verb stem (e.g., jump). Single-mechanism accounts claim that the deficits reflect a general phonological impairment that affects perception of regular past tense offsets but which might preserve normal activation of verb semantics. We tested four patients with regular past tense deficits and matched controls, using a paired auditory semantic priming/lexical decision task with three conditions: uninflected verbs (hope/wish), regular past tense primes (blamed/accuse), and irregular past tense primes (shook/tremble). Both groups showed significant priming for verbs with simple morphophonology (uninflected verbs and irregular past tenses) but the patients showed no priming for verbs with complex morphophonology (regular past tenses) in contrast to controls. The findings suggest that the patients are delayed in activating the meaning of verbs if a regular past tense affix is appended, consistent with a dual-route account of their deficit.

[1]  W. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Rules, representations, and the English past tense , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[2]  L. K. Tyler,et al.  Automatic Access of Lexical Information in Broca′s Aphasics: Against the Automaticity Hypothesis , 1995, Brain and Language.

[3]  S. Pinker,et al.  On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition , 1988, Cognition.

[4]  M. Ullman A neurocognitive perspective on language: The declarative/procedural model , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[5]  William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Phonology and neuropsychology of the English past tense , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  Sheila E. Blumstein,et al.  Mapping from Sound to Meaning: Reduced Lexical Activation in Broca's Aphasics , 2001, Brain and Language.

[7]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Deficits in phonology and past-tense morphology: What's the connection? , 2003 .

[8]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Basic processes in reading : visual word recognition , 1993 .

[9]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Deficits in irregular past-tense verb morphology associated with degraded semantic knowledge , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  B. J. Winer Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 1992 .

[11]  Kenneth I Forster,et al.  DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy , 2003, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[12]  William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Activating Verb Semantics from the Regular and Irregular Past Tense , 2001 .

[13]  J. H. Neely Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. , 1991 .

[14]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Dissociation of inflectional and derivational morphology , 1988, Brain and Language.

[15]  S. Pinker,et al.  A Neural Dissociation within Language: Evidence that the Mental Dictionary Is Part of Declarative Memory, and that Grammatical Rules Are Processed by the Procedural System , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[16]  A. Marcel,et al.  Aphasia, dyslexia and the phonological coding of written words. , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[17]  P. Hagoort Semantic Priming in Broca's Aphasics at a Short SOA: No Support for an Automatic Access Deficit , 1997, Brain and Language.

[18]  James L. McClelland,et al.  On learning the past-tenses of English verbs: implicit rules or parallel distributed processing , 1986 .

[19]  D. Swinney,et al.  Neural correlates of lexicon and grammar: Evidence from the production, reading, and judgment of inflection in aphasia , 2005, Brain and Language.

[20]  Lorraine K. Tyler,et al.  A Distributed Memory Model of the Associative Boost in Semantic Priming , 1994, Connect. Sci..

[21]  R. H. Baayen,et al.  The CELEX Lexical Database (CD-ROM) , 1996 .

[22]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Voxel-Based Morphometry of Herpes Simplex Encephalitis , 2001, NeuroImage.

[23]  K. Forster,et al.  Lexical storage and retrieval of prefixed words , 1975 .

[24]  William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Dissociations in Processing Past Tense Morphology: Neuropathology and Behavioral Studies , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  R. Schreuder,et al.  War and Peace: Morphemes and Full Forms in a Noninteractive Activation Parallel Dual-Route Model , 1999, Brain and Language.

[26]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Lexical access and inflectional morphology , 1988, Cognition.

[27]  H. Clahsen,et al.  Lexical entries and rules of language: A multidisciplinary study of German inflection , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[28]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  A voxel‐based morphometry study of semantic dementia: Relationship between temporal lobe atrophy and semantic memory , 2000, Annals of neurology.

[29]  Steven Pinker,et al.  Generalizations of regular and irregular morphology , 1993 .

[30]  S. Pinker,et al.  The past and future of the past tense , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[31]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  Methods for Dealing With Reaction Time Outliers , 1992 .

[32]  William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Dissociating types of mental computation , 1997, Nature.

[33]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Impairments in verb morphology after brain injury: a connectionist model. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[34]  Sheila E. Blumstein,et al.  A Perspective on the Neurobiology of Language , 1997, Brain and Language.

[35]  B MacWhinney,et al.  Frequency and the lexical storage of regularly inflected forms , 1986, Memory & cognition.

[36]  J. Berko,et al.  Agrammatism and inflectional morphology in English. , 1960, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[37]  J. Herskowitz,et al.  Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA , 1996, Current Biology.

[38]  S. Blumstein,et al.  Lexical decision and aphasia: Evidence for semantic processing , 1981, Brain and Language.

[39]  W. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Capturing underlying differentiation in the human language system , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[40]  Lorraine K Tyler,et al.  Real-time comprehension processes in agrammatism: A case study , 1985, Brain and Language.

[41]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Rules or connections in past-tense inflections: what does the evidence rule out? , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.