Understanding the points of fables

This article proposes a model of the reader's understanding of the points of simple narrative texts, such as fables, and tests this model by using point generation and fable comprehension data from adult subjects. The model identifies three components necessary for understanding the fable's moral or point: (a) the positive or negative valence of the central action in the fable, (b) the positive or negative valence of the fable's outcome, and (c) the consistency in valence between action and outcome information. The model was tested by having subjects read traditional Aesop's fables and three types of experimental fables written to be inconsistent with the proposed fable schema. The data showed strong support for the theory. Readers had considerable difficulty deriving points for texts that were inconsistent with the proposed schema, and, in those cases in which the data were not in agreement with the predictions, it was possible to use components of the model to provide an account of the basic findings.

[1]  L. Polanyi,et al.  So What’s The Point? , 1979 .

[2]  Wendy G. Lehnert Plot Units and Narrative Summarization , 1981 .

[3]  P. Thorndyke Cognitive structures in comprehension and memory of narrative discourse , 1977, Cognitive Psychology.

[4]  William F. Brewer Plan Understanding, Narrative Comprehension, and Story Schemas , 1982, AAAI.

[5]  Gordon H. Bower,et al.  Experiments on story comprehension and recall , 1978 .

[6]  W. Kintsch,et al.  Strategies of discourse comprehension , 1983 .

[7]  Nancy L. Stein,et al.  The effects of organization and instructional set on story memory , 1978 .

[8]  D. Rumelhart NOTES ON A SCHEMA FOR STORIES , 1975 .

[9]  J. Mandler,et al.  On the psychological validity of story structure , 1982 .

[10]  William F. Brewer,et al.  Event schemas, story schemas, and story grammars , 1980 .

[11]  Bertram C. Bruce A social interaction model of reading , 1981 .

[12]  T. Trabasso,et al.  Causal thinking and the representation of narrative events , 1985 .

[13]  Russell A. Hunt,et al.  Point-driven understanding: Pragmatic and cognitive dimensions of literary reading , 1984 .

[14]  M. Lerner,et al.  The Belief in a Just World , 1980 .

[15]  R. Gerrig,et al.  Suspense in the absence of uncertainty , 1989 .

[16]  William F. Brewer,et al.  Development of story liking: Character identification, suspense, and outcome resolution. , 1984 .

[17]  N. S. Johnson,et al.  Remembrance of things parsed: Story structure and recall , 1977, Cognitive Psychology.

[18]  Paul van den Broek,et al.  Causal networks versus goal hierarchies in summarizing text , 1986 .

[19]  T. Trabasso,et al.  Logical necessity and transitivity of causal relations in stories , 1989 .

[20]  Suzanne Hidi,et al.  Interestingness - A Neglected Variable in Discourse Processing , 1986, Cogn. Sci..

[21]  William F. Brewer,et al.  Form, content, and affect in the theory of stories , 1983, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[22]  J. G. Holmes,et al.  Deserving and the Emergence of Forms of Justice1 , 1976 .

[23]  Mandler JeanM. A code in the node: The use of a story schema in retrieval , 1978 .

[24]  Jean M. Mandler,et al.  A tale of two structures: Underlying and surface forms in stories , 1980 .

[25]  D. Waltz,et al.  The holes in points , 1983, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[26]  Roger C. Schank,et al.  What's the point? , 1982 .

[27]  Robert de Beaugrande,et al.  Narrative Models of Action and Interaction , 1979, Cogn. Sci..

[28]  R. Wilensky Story grammars versus story points , 1983, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[29]  Walter Kintsch,et al.  Toward a model of text comprehension and production. , 1978 .

[30]  William F. Brewer,et al.  Stories Are to Entertain: A Structural-Affect Theory of Stories. Technical Report No. 265. , 1982 .

[31]  Wayne C. Booth,et al.  A Rhetoric of Irony , 1975 .

[32]  P. Broek The effects of causal relations and hierarchical position on the importance of story statements , 1988 .