Infant-Directed Speech Facilitates Word Segmentation.

There are reasons to believe that infant-directed (ID) speech may make language acquisition easier for infants. However, the effects of ID speech on infants' learning remain poorly understood. The experiments reported here assess whether ID speech facilitates word segmentation from fluent speech. One group of infants heard a set of nonsense sentences spoken with intonation contours characteristic of adult-directed (AD) speech, and the other group heard the same sentences spoken with intonation contours characteristic of ID speech. In both cases, the only cue to word boundaries was the statistical structure of the speech. Infants were able to distinguish words from syllable sequences spanning word boundaries after exposure to ID speech but not after hearing AD speech. These results suggest that ID speech facilitates word segmentation and may be useful for other aspects of language acquisition as well. Issues of direction of preference in preferential listening paradigms are also considered.

[1]  L. Frank The Society for Research in Child Development , 1935 .

[2]  J. Hayes Cognition and the development of language , 1970 .

[3]  H. Gleitman,et al.  The current status of the motherese hypothesis , 1984, Journal of Child Language.

[4]  A. Fernald Four-Month-Old Infants Prefer to Listen to Motherese" , 1985 .

[5]  Elissa L Newport,et al.  Structural packaging in the input to language learning: Contributions of prosodic and morphological marking of phrases to the acquisition of language , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[6]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Acoustic determinants of infant preference for motherese speech , 1987 .

[7]  E. W. Ames,et al.  A multifactor model of infant preferences for novel and familiar stimuli. , 1988 .

[8]  A. Fernald,et al.  Intonation and communicative intent in mothers' speech to infants: is the melody the message? , 1989, Child development.

[9]  Bambi B. Schieffelin,et al.  The Give and Take of Everyday Life: Language Socialization of Kaluli Children , 1992 .

[10]  A. Fernald,et al.  Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults , 1991 .

[11]  S. Rude,et al.  Depressive deficits in memory: focusing attention improves subsequent recall. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[12]  A. Fernald Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. , 1992 .

[13]  R. Aslin,et al.  Developmental differences in infant attention to the spectral properties of infant-directed speech. , 1994, Child development.

[14]  Janet F. Werker,et al.  A cross-language investigation of infant preference for infant-directed communication☆ , 1994 .

[15]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Infants′ Detection of the Sound Patterns of Words in Fluent Speech , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.

[16]  R. Golinkoff,et al.  Infant-directed speech facilitates lexical learning in adults hearing Chinese: implications for language acquisition , 1995, Journal of Child Language.

[17]  J. Cohn,et al.  A combination of vocal fo dynamic and summary features discriminates between three pragmatic categories of infant-directed speech. , 1996, Child development.

[18]  P. Kaplan,et al.  Infant-Directed versus Adult-Directed Speech as Signals for Faces. , 1996 .

[19]  Agaath M. C. Sluijter,et al.  Spectral balance as an acoustic correlate of linguistic stress. , 1996, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[20]  M. Goldsmith,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996 .

[21]  C. Fisher,et al.  Acoustic cues to grammatical structure in infant-directed speech: cross-linguistic evidence. , 1996, Child development.

[22]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants. , 1997, Science.

[23]  M. Raijmakers Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. , 1997 .

[24]  E. Newport,et al.  Computation of Conditional Probability Statistics by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1998 .

[25]  John P. Pinto,et al.  Rapid Gains in Speed of Verbal Processing by Infants in the 2nd Year , 1998 .

[26]  Ronald A. Rensink,et al.  On the Failure to Detect Changes in Scenes Across Brief Interruptions , 2000 .

[27]  L. Trainor,et al.  Is Infant-Directed Speech Prosody a Result of the Vocal Expression of Emotion? , 2000, Psychological science.

[28]  John E. Richards,et al.  Individual Differences in Infants' Recognition of Briefly Presented Visual Stimuli. , 1998, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[29]  J. Bachorowski,et al.  Role of Clinical Diagnosis and Medication Use in Effects of Maternal Depression on Infant-Directed Speech. , 2001, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[30]  Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al.  Word Segmentation by 8-Month-Olds: When Speech Cues Count More Than Statistics , 2001 .

[31]  J. Bachorowski,et al.  Infants of Depressed Mothers, Although Competent Learners, Fail to Learn in Response to Their Own Mothers' Infant-Directed Speech , 2002, Psychological science.

[32]  Leher Singh,et al.  Infants' Listening Preferences: Baby Talk or Happy Talk? , 2002, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[33]  L. Trainor,et al.  Pitch characteristics of infant-directed speech affect infants’ ability to discriminate vowels , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[34]  Erik D. Thiessen,et al.  When cues collide: use of stress and statistical cues to word boundaries by 7- to 9-month-old infants. , 2003, Developmental psychology.

[35]  Patricia K. Kuhl,et al.  Fast Track , 2020, Electric and Hybrid Vehicle Technology International.

[36]  J. Feldman,et al.  Infant visual recognition memory: independent contributions of speed and attention. , 2003, Developmental psychology.