Top-down modulation of the perception of other people in schizophrenia and autism

Accurately and efficiently perceiving social cues such as body movements and facial expressions is important in social interaction. Accurate social perception of this kind does not solely rely on “bottom-up” visual processing but is also subject to modulation by “top-down” signals. For example, if instructed to look for signs of happiness rather than fear, participants are more likely to categorize facial expressions as happy—this prior expectation biases subsequent perception. Top-down modulation is also important in our reactions to others. For example, top-down control over imitation plays an important role in the development of smooth and harmonious social interactions. This paper highlights the importance of top-down modulation in our perception of, and reactions to, others. We discuss evidence that top-down modulation of social perception and imitation is atypical in Autism Spectrum Conditions and in schizophrenia, and we consider the effect this may have on the development of social interactions for individuals with these developmental disorders.

[1]  F. Lhermitte,et al.  Human autonomy and the frontal lobes. Part II: Patient behavior in complex and social situations: The “environmental dependency syndrome” , 1986, Annals of neurology.

[2]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Posterior parietal cortex in rhesus monkey: II. Evidence for segregated corticocortical networks linking sensory and limbic areas with the frontal lobe , 1989, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[3]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A PET study of visuospatial attention , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[4]  B. Cornblatt,et al.  Impaired attention, genetics, and the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. , 1994, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[5]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  The Helmholtz Machine , 1995, Neural Computation.

[6]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Schizophrenia: a disconnection syndrome? , 1995, Clinical neuroscience.

[7]  C D Frith,et al.  Space-based and object-based visual attention: shared and specific neural domains. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[8]  D. Penn,et al.  Cognition and social functioning in schizophrenia. , 1997, Psychiatry.

[9]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The functional anatomy of attention to visual motion. A functional MRI study. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[10]  H. Brownell,et al.  Acquired `theory of mind' impairments following stroke , 1999, Cognition.

[11]  B. Dosher,et al.  Mechanisms of perceptual learning , 1999, Vision Research.

[12]  T. Chartrand,et al.  The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[13]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. , 1999, Science.

[14]  B. Dosher,et al.  Mechanisms of perceptual learning , 1999, Vision Research.

[15]  N. Tarrier,et al.  Scales to measure dimensions of hallucinations and delusions: the psychotic symptom rating scales (PSYRATS) , 1999, Psychological Medicine.

[16]  C. Frith,et al.  Movement and Mind: A Functional Imaging Study of Perception and Interpretation of Complex Intentional Movement Patterns , 2000, NeuroImage.

[17]  Adrian M. Owen,et al.  The role of the lateral frontal cortex in mnemonic processing: the contribution of functional neuroimaging , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  H. Critchley,et al.  The functional neuroanatomy of social behaviour: changes in cerebral blood flow when people with autistic disorder process facial expressions. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[19]  F. Volkmar,et al.  Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity during face discrimination among individuals with autism and Asperger syndrome. , 2000, Archives of general psychiatry.

[20]  J. M. Anderson,et al.  Responses of human frontal cortex to surprising events are predicted by formal associative learning theory , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  Robin M. Murray,et al.  Cognitive neuropsychiatric models of persecutory delusions. , 2001, The American journal of psychiatry.

[22]  R. Dolan,et al.  Effects of Attention and Emotion on Face Processing in the Human Brain An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2001, Neuron.

[23]  Marcel Brass,et al.  The Inhibition of Imitative Response Tendencies , 2001, NeuroImage.

[24]  E. Courchesne,et al.  Face processing occurs outside the fusiform 'face area' in autism: evidence from functional MRI. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  The neural basis of biased competition in human visual cortex , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  E. Irle,et al.  Emotional priming of facial affect identification in schizophrenia. , 2001, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[27]  K. Kiehl,et al.  An event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study of an auditory oddball task in schizophrenia , 2001, Schizophrenia Research.

[28]  J. Ford,et al.  Reduced communication between frontal and temporal lobes during talking in schizophrenia , 2002, Biological Psychiatry.

[29]  C. Frith,et al.  Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[30]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Attentional control of the processing of neural and emotional stimuli. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[31]  Leanne M Williams,et al.  Visual scanpaths to positive and negative facial emotions in an outpatient schizophrenia sample , 2002, Schizophrenia Research.

[32]  Robin M. Chan,et al.  An fMRI study of facial emotion processing in patients with schizophrenia. , 2002, The American journal of psychiatry.

[33]  C. Frith What do imaging studies tell us about the neural basis of autism? , 2003, Novartis Foundation symposium.

[34]  Riitta Hari,et al.  Impaired Mirror-Image Imitation in Asperger and High-Functioning Autistic Subjects , 2003, Current Biology.

[35]  T. Chartrand,et al.  Using Nonconscious Behavioral Mimicry to Create Affiliation and Rapport , 2003, Psychological science.

[36]  Jan Derrfuss,et al.  Imitative response tendencies in patients with frontal brain lesions. , 2003, Neuropsychology.

[37]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  The essential role of Broca's area in imitation , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[38]  S. Hepburn,et al.  Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. , 2003, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[39]  T. Suslow,et al.  Affective priming in schizophrenia with and without affective negative symptoms , 2003, European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience.

[40]  André Aleman,et al.  Cognitive basis of hallucinations in schizophrenia: role of top-down information processing , 2003, Schizophrenia Research.

[41]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Projections from inferior temporal cortex to prefrontal cortex via the uncinate fascicle in rhesus monkeys , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Prediction Error during Retrospective Revaluation of Causal Associations in Humans fMRI Evidence in Favor of an Associative Model of Learning , 2004, Neuron.

[43]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[44]  A. van Knippenberg,et al.  Mimicry and Prosocial Behavior , 2004, Psychological science.

[45]  T. Robbins,et al.  The role of the lateral frontal cortex in causal associative learning: exploring preventative and super-learning. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[46]  Christopher F. Chabris,et al.  Activation of the fusiform gyrus when individuals with autism spectrum disorder view faces , 2004, NeuroImage.

[47]  M. Iacoboni Neural mechanisms of imitation , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[48]  Uta Frith,et al.  Theory of mind , 2001, Current Biology.

[49]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A theory of cortical responses , 2005, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[50]  Aysenil Belger,et al.  Imaging frontostriatal function in ultra-high-risk, early, and chronic schizophrenia during executive processing. , 2005, Archives of general psychiatry.

[51]  Joseph P. McCleery,et al.  EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[52]  Jennifer H. Pfeifer,et al.  Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[53]  P. Winkielman,et al.  When the social mirror breaks: deficits in automatic, but not voluntary, mimicry of emotional facial expressions in autism. , 2006, Developmental science.

[54]  T. Woodward,et al.  The Contribution of a Cognitive Bias Against Disconfirmatory Evidence (BADE) to Delusions in Schizophrenia , 2006, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[55]  P. Dayan,et al.  Cortical substrates for exploratory decisions in humans , 2006, Nature.

[56]  Geoffrey Bird,et al.  Attention does not modulate neural responses to social stimuli in autism spectrum disorders , 2006, NeuroImage.

[57]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  The Role of the Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex in Abstract State-Based Inference during Decision Making in Humans , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[58]  Jennifer A. Mangels,et al.  Predictive Codes for Forthcoming Perception in the Frontal Cortex , 2006, Science.

[59]  Steffen Moritz,et al.  A generalized bias against disconfirmatory evidence in schizophrenia , 2006, Psychiatry Research.

[60]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Frontal responses during learning predict vulnerability to the psychotogenic effects of ketamine: linking cognition, brain activity, and psychosis. , 2006, Archives of general psychiatry.

[61]  D. Freeman Suspicious minds: the psychology of persecutory delusions. , 2007, Clinical psychology review.

[62]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Disrupted prediction-error signal in psychosis: evidence for an associative account of delusions. , 2007, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[63]  T. Woodward,et al.  A bias against disconfirmatory evidence is associated with delusion proneness in a nonclinical sample. , 2007, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[64]  C. Heyes,et al.  Intact automatic imitation of human and robot actions in autism spectrum disorders , 2007, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[65]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Auditory processing in schizophrenia during the middle latency period (10-50 ms): high-density electrical mapping and source analysis reveal subcortical antecedents to early cortical deficits. , 2007, Journal of psychiatry & neuroscience : JPN.

[66]  Simon Surguladze,et al.  Facial fear processing and psychotic symptoms in schizophrenia: functional magnetic resonance imaging study , 2008, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[67]  C. Summerfield,et al.  A Neural Representation of Prior Information during Perceptual Inference , 2008, Neuron.

[68]  Steffen Moritz,et al.  Belief inflexibility in schizophrenia , 2008, Cognitive neuropsychiatry.

[69]  David L. Roberts,et al.  Social Cognition in Schizophrenia: An Overview , 2007, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[70]  A. Belger,et al.  Atypical modulation of cognitive control by arousal in autism , 2008, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.

[71]  Marcel Brass,et al.  Control of shared representations relies on key processes involved in mental state attribution , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[72]  M. Iacoboni Imitation, empathy, and mirror neurons. , 2009, Annual review of psychology.

[73]  Caroline Catmur,et al.  Associative sequence learning: the role of experience in the development of imitation and the mirror system , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[74]  C. Frith,et al.  From drugs to deprivation: a Bayesian framework for understanding models of psychosis , 2009, Psychopharmacology.

[75]  C. Summerfield,et al.  Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[76]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dysconnection in Schizophrenia: From Abnormal Synaptic Plasticity to Failures of Self-monitoring , 2009, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[77]  Alexander Kraskov,et al.  Corticospinal Neurons in Macaque Ventral Premotor Cortex with Mirror Properties: A Potential Mechanism for Action Suppression? , 2009, Neuron.

[78]  M. Just,et al.  From the Selectedworks of Marcel Adam Just Atypical Frontal-posterior Synchronization of Theory of Mind Regions in Autism during Mental State Attribution Atypical Frontal-posterior Synchronization of Theory of Mind Regions in Autism during Mental State Attribution , 2022 .

[79]  ChrisD . Frith,et al.  Perceiving is believing: a Bayesian approach to explaining the positive symptoms of schizophrenia , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[80]  M. Brass,et al.  Hyperimitation of Actions Is Related to Reduced Understanding of Others' Minds in Autism Spectrum Conditions , 2010, Biological Psychiatry.

[81]  Ralph-Axel Müller,et al.  Atypical network connectivity for imitation in autism spectrum disorder , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[82]  J. Gomez,et al.  When seeing depends on knowing: Adults with Autism Spectrum Conditions show diminished top-down processes in the visual perception of degraded faces but not degraded objects , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[83]  Geoffrey Bird,et al.  Social attitudes modulate automatic imitation , 2010 .

[84]  A. Hamilton,et al.  Eye contact enhances mimicry of intransitive hand movements , 2011, Biology Letters.

[85]  Daniel C. Richardson,et al.  Intact imitation of emotional facial actions in autism spectrum conditions , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[86]  Marcel Brass,et al.  Resisting motor mimicry: Control of imitation involves processes central to social cognition in patients with frontal and temporo-parietal lesions , 2010, Social neuroscience.

[87]  L. Pessoa,et al.  Emotion processing and the amygdala: from a 'low road' to 'many roads' of evaluating biological significance , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[88]  Aysenil Belger,et al.  Impaired modulation of attention and emotion in schizophrenia. , 2010, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[89]  P. Allen,et al.  Attentional modulation of external speech attribution in patients with hallucinations and delusions , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[90]  E. Walker,et al.  Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders , 2013 .

[91]  P. Fletcher,et al.  Glutamatergic Model Psychoses: Prediction Error, Learning, and Inference , 2011, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[92]  Christopher Summerfield,et al.  Dissociable prior influences of signal probability and relevance on visual contrast sensitivity , 2011 .

[93]  Sara C. Verosky,et al.  Can I trust you? Negative affective priming influences social judgments in schizophrenia. , 2011, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[94]  Richard Ramsey,et al.  The Control of Mimicry by Eye Contact Is Mediated by Medial Prefrontal Cortex , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[95]  G. Bird,et al.  Social attitudes differentially modulate imitation in adolescents and adults , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[96]  The influence of prior expectations on facial expression discrimination in schizophrenia – ERRATUM , 2012, Psychological Medicine.

[97]  G. Bird,et al.  Atypical Social Modulation of Imitation in Autism Spectrum Conditions , 2011, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[98]  S. Blakemore,et al.  The influence of prior expectations on facial expression discrimination in schizophrenia , 2012, Psychological Medicine.

[99]  R. Schultz,et al.  The social motivation theory of autism , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[100]  Janet B W Williams,et al.  Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders , 2013 .

[101]  Sarah-Jayne Blakemore,et al.  The influence of prior expectations on emotional face perception in adolescence. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[102]  N. Andreasen Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms , 2014 .