The influence of response conflict on voluntary task switching: a novel test of the conflict monitoring model

The conflict monitoring model of cognitive control posits that response conflict triggers a top-down enhancement of a task’s representation in working memory. In the present study, we conducted a novel test of the conflict monitoring model using a voluntary task switching paradigm. We predicted that a task’s representation would be enhanced following events associated with high response conflict (i.e., incongruent trials and incorrect responses), leading participants to voluntarily choose to repeat that task more often after these events than after events associated with low response conflict (i.e., congruent trials and correct responses). In two experiments, performance following incongruent trials was consistent with the conflict monitoring model. However, performance following incorrect trials did not fit with the model’s predictions. These findings provide novel support for the conflict monitoring model while revealing new effects of incorrect trials that the model cannot explain.

[1]  R. D. Gordon,et al.  Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. , 2001, Psychological review.

[2]  Catherine M Arrington,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article The Cost of a Voluntary Task Switch , 2022 .

[3]  Richard Ridderinkhof Micro- and macro-adjustments of task set: activation and suppression in conflict tasks , 2002, Psychological research.

[4]  M. Schmitter-Edgecombe,et al.  Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks following severe closed-head injury. , 2006, Neuropsychology.

[5]  Borís Burle,et al.  Executive control in the Simon effect: an electromyographic and distributional analysis , 2002, Psychological research.

[6]  W. Notebaert,et al.  Dissociating conflict adaptation from feature integration: a multiple regression approach. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  S. Monsell,et al.  Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .

[8]  E. Awh,et al.  Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  J. Meere The role of attention. , 2002 .

[10]  P. Rabbitt Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[11]  N. Meiran Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. , 1996 .

[12]  Cameron S Carter,et al.  Generalized signaling for control: evidence from postconflict and posterror performance adjustments. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  E. Donchin,et al.  Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[14]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  Voluntary task switching under load: Contribution of top-down and bottom-up factors in goal-directed behavior , 2010, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[15]  Ulrich Mayr,et al.  On How to be Unpredictable : Evidence from the Voluntary Task-Switching Paradigm , 2005 .

[16]  W. Notebaert,et al.  Top-down and bottom-up sequential modulations of congruency effects , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[17]  Catherine M. Arrington,et al.  Voluntary Task Switching , 2014 .

[18]  David E. Meyer,et al.  Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. , 2001 .

[19]  Donald Laming,et al.  Information theory of choice-reaction times , 1968 .

[20]  Switch M. Arrington,et al.  The Cost of a Voluntary Task , 2004 .

[21]  D. Meyer,et al.  Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[23]  Catherine M Arrington,et al.  Voluntary task switching: chasing the elusive homunculus. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[24]  T. Egner Multiple conflict-driven control mechanisms in the human brain , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  M. Botvinick Conflict monitoring and decision making: Reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[26]  Catherine M Arrington,et al.  Stimulus-based priming of task choice during voluntary task switching. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[27]  A. Henik,et al.  Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks , 1982, Memory & cognition.

[28]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. , 2004, Psychological review.

[29]  Jacob Cohen,et al.  Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences , 1979 .

[30]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[31]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.

[32]  T. Goschke Intentional reconfiguration and involuntary persistence in task-set switching , 2000 .

[33]  J. Driver,et al.  Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , 2000 .

[34]  Jeremy R. Reynolds,et al.  A computational model of fractionated conflict-control mechanisms in task-switching , 2007, Cognitive Psychology.

[35]  W. Notebaert,et al.  Cognitive control acts locally , 2008, Cognition.

[36]  S. Keele,et al.  Changing internal constraints on action: the role of backward inhibition. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[37]  B. Hommel,et al.  A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task , 2004, Psychological research.

[38]  Nachshon Meiran,et al.  Central interference in error processing , 2005, Memory & cognition.

[39]  Catherine M. Arrington,et al.  The role of attentional networks in voluntary task switching , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[40]  D. Alan Allport,et al.  SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .

[41]  Eric Ruthruff,et al.  Inhibition of task set: Converging evidence from task choice in the voluntary task-switching paradigm , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[42]  Joseph T. McGuire,et al.  Prefrontal cortex, cognitive control, and the registration of decision costs , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[43]  A Baddeley,et al.  Random Generation and the Executive Control of Working Memory , 1998, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[44]  E. Davelaar Conflict-monitoring and (meta)cognitive control , 2009 .

[45]  Lauren M. Bylsma,et al.  The conflict adaptation effect: It’s not just priming , 2005, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[46]  Bernhard Hommel,et al.  Feature integration across perception and action: event files affect response choice , 2007, Psychological research.

[47]  Carolin Dudschig,et al.  Short Article: Why do we slow down after an error? Mechanisms underlying the effects of posterror slowing , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[48]  B. Hommel Event Files: Evidence for Automatic Integration of Stimulus-Response Episodes , 1998 .