Syntactic Bootstrapping in the Acquisition of Attitude Verbs

Attitude verbs, like think, want and hope, seem to be acquired later than other verbs. Moreover, some attitude verbs (want) seem to be acquired later than others (think). Why should this be? Perhaps observability is a bigger problem than in other cases of word-learning. Psychological events like thinking and wanting do not have direct physical correlates unlike objects like dog or cat, or events like running or eating. Although on the surface, this seems to be a huge problem, it may not be as difficult as it initially appears to be. Advances in cognitive development have revealed that from a very young age, all humans share common conceptual and perceptual capacities, which have been argued to weaken the force of Quine’s famous induction problem. From just a few months old, infants have rich concepts of continuity of objects, gravity, event representation and more (Carey 2009, Eimas & Miller 1990, Soja, Carey & Spelke 1991, Spleke & Kestenbaum 1986, and others), and these concepts shape their perception and attention in a way that may restrict candidate word meanings. In addition, infants have been shown to track the minds of others with astonishing ease. From the first months of life, infants give privileged status to human agents, and are sensitive to conversation partners’ goals and perspectives. They attribute goal-directedness to human agents from as young as 5 months of age (Woodward 1996). This sensitivity to other people’s minds also aids children in language acquisition. Children can track eyegaze and use it to learn new words by 16 months old (Baldwin 1991, Bloom 2000, Plunkett 1997). By age 2, children are adult-like in their interpretation of indexical pronouns, which shift reference based on conversational roles (Moyer, Harrigan, Hacquard & Lidz 2014). Given the salience of psychological states in infant reasoning, linking psychological concepts with words may be no more difficult than linking object and event concepts with words, despite the lack of physical evidence in the world. If, due to this richness of the learners’ representations of other minds, this linking is indeed straightforward, then there is no conceptual barrier to learning attitude verbs. Indeed, some attitude verbs do seem to be acquired easily. Children start producing want to express desires as young as 18 months (Bartsch & Wellman 1995). They further seem to be very good at understanding sentences with want by around 3 years of age (Bartsch & Wellman 1995; Harrigan, Hacquard & Lidz, submitted; Repacholi & Gopnik 1997; Wellman & Banerjee 1991; Wellman & Bartsch 1988; Wellman & Wooley 1990, and others). This suggests that the concept of desire is readily available to very young children, that they are proficient from a very young age at tracking others’ minds, and that their sophisticated cognitive capacities allow them to link the word want to the concept of desire with relative ease. For the verb think, however, we see a different trajectory. Many studies have found that children have difficulty understanding sentences with think well into their fourth year, in particular when the sentences refer to someone’s false belief (de Villiers 2005; 2007; de Villiers & de Villiers 2000; de Villiers & Pyers 2002; Johnson & Maratsos 1977; Lewis 2013; Perner et al. 2003;

[1]  A. Leslie,et al.  Attending to and learning about mental states , 2000 .

[2]  H. Rakoczy Executive function and the development of belief-desire psychology. , 2009, Developmental science.

[3]  J. Hintikka Semantics for Propositional Attitudes , 1969 .

[4]  C. Fisher,et al.  Two-year-olds use distributional cues to interpret transitivity-alternating verbs , 2009, Language and cognitive processes.

[5]  G. Edelman,et al.  Signal and sense : local and global order in perceptual maps , 1990 .

[6]  de Villiers,et al.  Can Language Acquisition Give Children a Point of View , 2005 .

[7]  C. Moore,et al.  Social Understanding at the End of the First Year of Life , 1994 .

[8]  E. Spelke,et al.  Spatiotemporal continuity, smoothness of motion and object identity in infancy , 1995 .

[9]  Jennie E. Pyers,et al.  Complements to cognition: a longitudinal study of the relationship between complex syntax and false-belief-understanding , 2002 .

[10]  Sylvia Yuan,et al.  Syntactic bootstrapping. , 2010, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[11]  B. Landau Language and experience , 1985 .

[12]  D. Sperber,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Attribution of Beliefs by 13-Month-Old Infants , 2022 .

[13]  John H. Flavell,et al.  The development of children's knowledge about the mind: From cognitive connections to mental representations. , 1988 .

[14]  L. Gleitman,et al.  Language and Experience: Evidence from the Blind Child , 1988 .

[15]  E. Spelke,et al.  Ontological categories guide young children's inductions of word meaning: Object terms and substance terms , 1991, Cognition.

[16]  A. Papafragou,et al.  Scalar implicatures: experiments at the semantics–pragmatics interface , 2003, Cognition.

[17]  Helen Tager-Flusberg,et al.  The influence of language on theory of mind: a training study. , 2003, Developmental science.

[18]  A. Papafragou From scalar semantics to implicature: children's interpretation of aspectuals , 2006, Journal of Child Language.

[19]  Letitia R. Naigles,et al.  Syntactic Supports for Word Learning , 2008 .

[20]  H. Wellman,et al.  Children Talk About the Mind , 1995 .

[21]  Alison Gopnik,et al.  The ontogeny of common sense. , 1988 .

[22]  Jill de Villiers,et al.  The Role of Language in Theory of Mind Development. , 2014 .

[23]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Early reasoning about desires: evidence from 14- and 18-month-olds. , 1997, Developmental psychology.

[24]  D. Bickerton The roots of language , 2016 .

[25]  Letitia R. Naigles,et al.  The use of multiple frames in verb learning via syntactic bootstrapping , 1996, Cognition.

[26]  Hannes Rakoczy,et al.  Symposium on S. Butterfill and I. Apperly, "How to Construct a Minimal Theory of Mind" , 2013 .

[27]  Lawrence M. Solan,et al.  The Acquisition of Structural Restrictions on Anaphora , 1981 .

[28]  E. Kako,et al.  First contact in verb acquisition: defining a role for syntax. , 1993, Child development.

[29]  Jeffrey Lidz,et al.  The role of incremental parsing in syntactically conditioned word learning , 2017, Cognitive Psychology.

[30]  Josef Perner,et al.  Implicit Understanding of Belief , 1994 .

[31]  A. Leslie,et al.  Solving belief problems: toward a task analysis , 1998, Cognition.

[32]  Mandy Simons,et al.  Observations on embedding verbs, evidentiality, and presupposition , 2007 .

[33]  André Meinunger Interface restrictions on verb second , 2006 .

[34]  D. Bolinger,et al.  语言要略 = Aspects of Language , 1968 .

[35]  G. Morgan,et al.  Nicaraguan Sign Language and Theory of Mind: the issue of critical periods and abilities. , 2006, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[36]  A. Papafragou,et al.  Lexical, syntactic, and semantic-geometric factors in the acquisition of motion predicates. , 2014, Developmental psychology.

[37]  Ian A Apperly,et al.  Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? , 2009, Psychological review.

[38]  L. Gleitman The Structural Sources of Verb Meanings , 2020, Sentence First, Arguments Afterward.

[39]  L. Gleitman,et al.  Hard Words , 2005, Language in Development.

[40]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind” ? , 1985, Cognition.

[41]  Irene Heim,et al.  Presupposition Projection and the Semantics of Attitude Verbs , 1992, J. Semant..

[42]  S. Carey The Origin of Concepts , 2000 .

[43]  P. Bloom How children learn the meanings of words , 2000 .

[44]  K. Maier INQUIRY , 2007 .

[45]  H. Wellman,et al.  Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. , 2001, Child development.

[46]  M. Maratsos,et al.  Early comprehension of mental verbs: Think and know , 1977 .

[47]  Tatjana Scheffler Semantic operators in different dimensions , 2008 .

[48]  A. Papafragou,et al.  Children's Computation of Implicatures , 2004 .

[49]  V. Hacquard,et al.  Three-Year-Olds’ Understanding of Know and Think , 2015 .

[50]  H. Wellman,et al.  Mind and emotion: Children's understanding of the emotional consequences of beliefs and desires , 1991 .

[51]  C. Heyes False belief in infancy: a fresh look. , 2014, Developmental science.

[52]  S. Pinker Learnability and Cognition: The Acquisition of Argument Structure , 1989 .

[53]  Shevaun Lewis,et al.  Pragmatic enrichment in language processing and development , 2013 .

[54]  D. Bishop,et al.  Pragmatic tolerance: Implications for the acquisition of informativeness and implicature , 2011, Cognition.

[55]  H. Wellman,et al.  From simple desires to ordinary beliefs: The early development of everyday psychology , 1990, Cognition.

[56]  E. Spelke,et al.  Perception, ontology, and word meaning , 1992, Cognition.

[57]  Kristen Nicole Asplin Can complement frames help children learn the meaning of abstract verbs , 2002 .

[58]  Elisabeth Villalta,et al.  Spanish Subjunctive Clauses Require Ordered Alternatives , 2000 .

[59]  R. Baillargeon,et al.  False-belief understanding in infants , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[60]  Jeffrey Lidz,et al.  Syntactic Bootstrapping in theAcquisition of Attitude Verbs: Think, Want and Hope , 2016 .

[61]  Amanda L. Woodward,et al.  Infants’ developing understanding of the link between looker and object , 2003 .

[62]  J. Michael,et al.  Why Desire Reasoning is Developmentally Prior to Belief Reasoning , 2015 .

[63]  A. Giannakidou The Landscape of Polarity Items , 1997 .

[64]  P. Portner Situation theory and the semantics of propositional expressions , 1992 .

[65]  J. Perner Understanding the Representational Mind , 1993 .

[66]  H. Gleitman,et al.  Human simulations of vocabulary learning , 1999, Cognition.

[67]  B. Sodian,et al.  Continuity from an implicit to an explicit understanding of false belief from infancy to preschool age. , 2012, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[68]  Jeffrey Lidz,et al.  The semantics and pragmatics of belief reports in preschoolers , 2012 .

[69]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception , 1983, Cognition.

[70]  S. Carey The child as word learner , 1978 .

[71]  D. Farkas Intensional descriptions and the romance subjunctive mood , 1985 .

[72]  Rory T. Devine,et al.  Relations between false belief understanding and executive function in early childhood: a meta-analysis. , 2014, Child development.

[73]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Naming in young children: a dumb attentional mechanism? , 1996, Cognition.

[74]  Nicola Yuill,et al.  Young children's coordination of motive and outcome in judgements of satisfaction and morality , 1984 .

[75]  E. Spelke Nativism, empiricism, and the origins of knowledge , 1998 .

[76]  Kim Plunkett,et al.  Theories of early language acquisition , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[77]  E. Villalta Mood and gradability: an investigation of the subjunctive mood in Spanish , 2008 .

[78]  Daniel Vanderveken,et al.  Foundations of Illocutionary Logic , 1985 .

[79]  S. Carey,et al.  Infants’ Metaphysics: The Case of Numerical Identity , 1996, Cognitive Psychology.

[80]  J. J. Guajardo,et al.  Is agency skin-deep? Surface attributes influence infants' sensitivity to goal-directed action , 2004 .

[81]  P. Hammond Attitude problems. , 1994, Nursing times.

[82]  O. Van Word and Object , 1960 .

[83]  Henry M. Wellman,et al.  Young children's reasoning about beliefs , 1988, Cognition.

[84]  Rosalind Thornton,et al.  Investigations in universal grammar: A guide to experiments on the acquisition of syntax and semantics , 1998 .

[85]  Siobhan Chapman Logic and Conversation , 2005 .

[86]  B. MacWhinney The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk , 1992 .

[87]  Cynthia Fisher,et al.  On the semantic content of subcategorization frames , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[88]  Kevin J. Riggs,et al.  Children's Reasoning and the Mind , 2000 .

[89]  Hubert Truckenbrodt,et al.  On the semantic motivation of syntactic verb movement to C in German , 2006 .

[90]  C. Moore,et al.  Conflicting desire and the child's theory of mind , 1995 .

[91]  Letitia R. Naigles Manipulating the input: Studies in mental verb acquisition. , 2000 .

[92]  M. Tomasello,et al.  “This way!”, “No! That way!”—3-year olds know that two people can have mutually incompatible desires , 2007 .

[93]  H. Wellman,et al.  The acquisition of mental verbs: A systematic investigation of the first reference to mental state , 1983, Cognition.

[94]  L. Gleitman,et al.  When we think about thinking: The acquisition of belief verbs , 2007, Cognition.

[95]  G. Csibra,et al.  Action Anticipation Through Attribution of False Belief by 2-Year-Olds , 2007, Psychological science.

[96]  J. de Villiers The Interface of Language and Theory of Mind. , 2007, Lingua. International review of general linguistics. Revue internationale de linguistique generale.

[97]  Alan M. Leslie,et al.  Belief-desire reasoning as a process of selection , 2005, Cognitive Psychology.

[98]  Josef Perner,et al.  Want that is understood well before say that, think that, and false belief: a test of de Villiers's linguistic determinism on German-speaking children. , 2003, Child development.

[99]  Sylvia Yuan,et al.  “Really? She Blicked the Baby?” , 2009, Psychological science.

[100]  J. Trueswell,et al.  The developing constraints on parsing decisions: The role of lexical-biases and referential scenes in child and adult sentence processing , 2004, Cognitive Psychology.

[101]  Josef Perner,et al.  Pleased and surprised: Children's cognitive theory of emotion , 1991 .

[102]  J. Hooper On Assertive Predicates , 1975 .

[103]  A. Woodward Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach , 1998, Cognition.

[104]  A. Leslie,et al.  Inhibitory processing in the false belief task: Two conjectures , 1998 .

[105]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Infants' contribution to the achievement of joint reference. , 1991, Child development.

[106]  Sunny Shin,et al.  Do 15-Month-Old Infants Understand False Beliefs ? , 2005 .

[107]  V. Hacquard,et al.  Epistemics and attitudes , 2013 .

[108]  A. Woodward Infants' ability to distinguish between purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors , 1999 .

[109]  J. Perner,et al.  What Does "That" Have to Do with Point of View? Conflicting Desires and "Want" in German , 2005 .

[110]  Letitia R. Naigles,et al.  Children use syntax to learn verb meanings , 1990, Journal of Child Language.

[111]  J. Perner,et al.  Infants' Insight into the Mind: How Deep? , 2005, Science.

[112]  Nancy L. Stein,et al.  The Causal Organisation of Emotional Knowledge: A Developmental Study , 1989 .