Adaptation to Visual Feedback Delay Influences Visuomotor Learning
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] C. Bard,et al. Deafferentation and pointing with visual double-step perturbations , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[2] Masaya Hirashima,et al. Intermittent Visual Feedback Can Boost Motor Learning of Rhythmic Movements: Evidence for Error Feedback Beyond Cycles , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[3] Peter Thier,et al. Internalizing agency of self-action: perception of one's own hand movements depends on an adaptable prediction about the sensory action outcome. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[4] Hiroaki Gomi,et al. Multiple Motor Learning Strategies in Visuomotor Rotation , 2010, PloS one.
[5] J. Flanagan,et al. Learning and recall of incremental kinematic and dynamic sensorimotor transformations , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.
[6] Daichi Nozaki,et al. Cross talk in implicit assignment of error information during bimanual visuomotor learning. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.
[7] Gordon M. Redding,et al. Applications of prism adaptation: a tutorial in theory and method , 2005, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[8] P. Haggard,et al. Intentional action: Conscious experience and neural prediction , 2003, Consciousness and Cognition.
[9] R. F. Thompson,et al. Temporal specificity of long-term depression in parallel fiber--Purkinje synapses in rat cerebellar slice. , 1995, Learning & memory.
[10] R. Held,et al. PLASTICITY IN HUMAN SENSORIMOTOR CONTROL. , 1963, Science.
[11] G. Stelmach,et al. Adaptation to gradual as compared with sudden visuo-motor distortions , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.
[12] Virginia A. Diggles,et al. Rapid error correction during human arm movements: evidence for central monitoring. , 1984, Journal of motor behavior.
[13] Madeleine Schlag-Rey,et al. Voluntary action expands perceived duration of its sensory consequence , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.
[14] J. Krakauer,et al. Sensory prediction errors drive cerebellum-dependent adaptation of reaching. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.
[15] Michael A. Arbib,et al. The handbook of brain theory and neural networks , 1995, A Bradford book.
[16] Zoubin Ghahramani,et al. Computational principles of movement neuroscience , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[17] Masaya Hirashima,et al. Asymmetric Transfer of Visuomotor Learning between Discrete and Rhythmic Movements , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[18] Reza Shadmehr,et al. Learned dynamics of reaching movements generalize from dominant to nondominant arm. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[19] Reza Shadmehr,et al. Learning of action through adaptive combination of motor primitives , 2000, Nature.
[20] P. Haggard,et al. Voluntary action and conscious awareness , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[21] E. Todorov. Optimality principles in sensorimotor control , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.
[22] J. Krakauer,et al. An Implicit Plan Overrides an Explicit Strategy during Visuomotor Adaptation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[23] L. Christensen,et al. University of Birmingham Disruption of state estimation in the human lateral cerebellum , 2007 .
[24] M. A. Arbib,et al. Models of Trajectory Formation and Temporal Interaction of Reach and Grasp. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.
[25] Lennart Ljung,et al. System Identification: Theory for the User , 1987 .
[26] Michael I. Jordan,et al. Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[27] Sarah E. Criscimagna-Hemminger,et al. Size of error affects cerebellar contributions to motor learning. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.
[28] P. Montague,et al. Motor-Sensory Recalibration Leads to an Illusory Reversal of Action and Sensation , 2006, Neuron.
[29] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[30] D. Ostry,et al. Is Interlimb Transfer of Force-Field Adaptation a Cognitive Response to the Sudden Introduction of Load? , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[31] David C. Knill,et al. Humans use continuous visual feedback from the hand to control both the direction and distance of pointing movements , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.
[32] Britne A. Shabbott,et al. Learning a visuomotor rotation: simultaneous visual and proprioceptive information is crucial for visuomotor remapping , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[33] Konrad Paul Kording,et al. Relevance of error: what drives motor adaptation? , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.
[34] R. Miall,et al. Adaptation to visual feedback delays in manual tracking: evidence against the Smith Predictor model of human visually guided action , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[35] S. Kitazawa,et al. Effects of delayed visual information on the rate and amount of prism adaptation in the human , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[36] James L. Patton,et al. A Real-Time Haptic/Graphic Demonstration of how Error Augmentation can Enhance Learning , 2005, Proceedings of the 2005 IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Automation.
[37] R. C. Oldfield. THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .
[38] Michael I. Jordan,et al. An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.
[39] Mitsuo Kawato,et al. Cerebellum and motor control , 1998 .
[40] D. Wolpert,et al. Is the cerebellum a smith predictor? , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.
[41] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[42] Reza Shadmehr,et al. Learning from Sensory and Reward Prediction Errors during Motor Adaptation , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[43] Alex Simpkins,et al. System Identification: Theory for the User, 2nd Edition (Ljung, L.; 1999) [On the Shelf] , 2012, IEEE Robotics & Automation Magazine.
[44] M. Hinder,et al. The contribution of visual feedback to visuomotor adaptation: How much and when? , 2008, Brain Research.
[45] Stephan Riek,et al. Real-time error detection but not error correction drives automatic visuomotor adaptation , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[46] Hiroshi Imamizu,et al. Physical delay but not subjective delay determines learning rate in prism adaptation , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[47] D. Sparks,et al. Corollary discharge provides accurate eye position information to the oculomotor system. , 1983, Science.
[48] Kurt A. Thoroughman,et al. Motor adaptation to single force pulses: sensitive to direction but insensitive to within-movement pulse placement and magnitude. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[49] R. Shadmehr,et al. Interacting Adaptive Processes with Different Timescales Underlie Short-Term Motor Learning , 2006, PLoS biology.
[50] D. Whitaker,et al. Effect before Cause: Supramodal Recalibration of Sensorimotor Timing , 2009, PloS one.
[51] R. Shadmehr,et al. Cerebellar Contributions to Adaptive Control of Saccades in Humans , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[52] J. Krakauer,et al. Error correction, sensory prediction, and adaptation in motor control. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.
[53] Scott E. Bevans,et al. Effect of visual error size on saccade adaptation in monkey. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[54] Vincent S. Huang,et al. Persistence of motor memories reflects statistics of the learning event. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.