Reconstructing spectral cues for sound localization from responses to rippled noise stimuli

Human sound localization in the mid-saggital plane (elevation) relies on an analysis of the idiosyncratic spectral shape cues provided by the head and pinnae. However, because the actual free-field stimulus spectrum is a-priori unknown to the auditory system, the problem of extracting the elevation angle from the sensory spectrum is ill-posed. Here we test different spectral localization models by eliciting head movements toward broad-band noise stimuli with randomly shaped, rippled amplitude spectra emanating from a speaker at a fixed location, while varying the ripple bandwidth between 1.5 and 5.0 cycles/octave. Six listeners participated in the experiments. From the distributions of localization responses toward the individual stimuli, we estimated the listeners’ spectral-shape cues underlying their elevation percepts, by applying maximum-likelihood estimation. The reconstructed spectral cues resulted to be invariant to the considerable variation in ripple bandwidth, and for each listener they had a remarkable resemblance to the idiosyncratic head-related transfer functions (HRTFs). These results are not in line with models that rely on the detection of a single peak or notch in the amplitude spectrum, nor with a local analysis of first- and second-order spectral derivatives. Instead, our data support a model in which the auditory system performs a cross-correlation between the sensory input at the eardrum-auditory nerve, and stored representations of HRTF spectral shapes, to extract the perceived elevation angle.

[1]  A John Van Opstal,et al.  The influence of duration and level on human sound localization. , 2004, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[2]  R. Gilkey,et al.  Sound localization in noise: the effect of signal-to-noise ratio. , 1996, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[3]  Manfred R. Schroeder,et al.  Synthesis of low-peak-factor signals and binary sequences with low autocorrelation (Corresp.) , 1970, IEEE Trans. Inf. Theory.

[4]  A. John Van Opstal,et al.  Plasticity in human sound localization induced by compressed spatial vision , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[5]  Paul M. Hofman,et al.  Relearning sound localization with new ears , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[6]  Paul M. Hofman,et al.  Bayesian reconstruction of sound localization cues from responses to random spectra , 2002, Biological Cybernetics.

[7]  Robert A. Butler,et al.  The psychophysical basis of monaural localization , 1984, Hearing Research.

[8]  P M Hofman,et al.  Spectro-temporal factors in two-dimensional human sound localization. , 1998, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[9]  V R Algazi,et al.  Elevation localization and head-related transfer function analysis at low frequencies. , 2001, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[10]  R A Butler,et al.  The linkage between stimulus frequency and covert peak areas as it relates to monaural localization , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[11]  W M Hartmann,et al.  Auditory spectral discrimination and the localization of clicks in the sagittal plane. , 1993, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[12]  J. C. Middlebrooks Narrow-band sound localization related to external ear acoustics. , 1992, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[13]  A. V. van Opstal,et al.  Relearning Sound Localization with a New Ear , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  A. V. van Opstal,et al.  Pinna Cues Determine Orienting Response Modes to Synchronous Sounds in Elevation , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  H. Steven Colburn,et al.  Role of spectral detail in sound-source localization , 1998, Nature.

[16]  Robert A. Butler,et al.  Binaural localization: Influence of stimulus frequency and the linkage to covert peak areas , 1993, Hearing Research.

[17]  Masakazu Konishi,et al.  Mechanisms of sound localization in the barn owl (Tyto alba) , 1979, Journal of comparative physiology.

[18]  J. Blauert Spatial Hearing: The Psychophysics of Human Sound Localization , 1983 .

[19]  Rik J. Otte,et al.  Age-related Hearing Loss and Ear Morphology Affect Vertical but not Horizontal Sound-Localization Performance , 2013, Journal of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology.

[20]  F L Wightman,et al.  Headphone simulation of free-field listening. II: Psychophysical validation. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[21]  H. Takemoto,et al.  Mechanism for generating peaks and notches of head-related transfer functions in the median plane. , 2012, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[22]  A. John Van Opstal,et al.  The effect of head roll on perceived auditory zenith , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  J. C. Middlebrooks,et al.  Localization of brief sounds: effects of level and background noise. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[24]  M. Cynader,et al.  A computational theory of spectral cue localization , 1993 .

[25]  F L Wightman,et al.  Headphone simulation of free-field listening. I: Stimulus synthesis. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[26]  John van Opstal The Auditory System and Human Sound-Localization Behavior , 2016 .

[27]  J. C. Middlebrooks,et al.  Two-dimensional sound localization by human listeners. , 1990, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[28]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Precise recording of human eye movements , 1975, Vision Research.

[29]  M. P. Zwiers,et al.  A Spatial Hearing Deficit in Early-Blind Humans , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  D. M. Green,et al.  Sound localization by human listeners. , 1991, Annual review of psychology.

[31]  R A Butler,et al.  An analysis of the monaural displacement of sound in space , 1987, Perception & psychophysics.