Process dissociation, cognitive architecture, and response time: comments on Lindsay and Jacoby (1994)

Process dissociation is based on 2 assumptions about the processes being dissociated: invariance of the processes across situations, and stochastic independence of the processes. In a recent application of process dissociation to the Stroop task (D. S. Lindsay & L. L. Jacoby, 1994), both of those assumptions were violated. It is argued that these violations were due to (a) an oversimplification of the processing architecture that ignores common stages such as guessing and response selection, (b) an assumption that the more automatic process (word reading) dominates over the intended process (color naming) in determining responses, and (c) an assumption that switching from the more common speeded response instruction (measuring speed) to a deadline response instruction (measuring accuracy) does not change processing. General implications for applying process dissociation to dynamic tasks are discussed.

[1]  Richard Schweickert,et al.  A critical path generalization of the additive factor method: Analysis of a stroop task , 1978 .

[2]  R. H. Phaf,et al.  SLAM: A connectionist model for attention in visual selection tasks , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.

[3]  Colin M. Macleod Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[4]  G. Logan,et al.  When it helps to be misled: Facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in a Stroop-like task , 1979 .

[5]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Tests of the automaticity of reading: dilution of Stroop effects by color-irrelevant stimuli. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  T H Carr,et al.  Automaticity and cognitive anatomy: is word recognition "automatic"? , 1992, The American journal of psychology.

[7]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and cognitive control. , 1975 .

[8]  Marilyn Bohl,et al.  Information processing , 1971 .

[9]  S.M. Harris,et al.  Information Processing , 1977, Nature.

[10]  James L. McClelland,et al.  On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. , 1990, Psychological review.

[11]  T H Carr,et al.  Automaticity and word perception: evidence from Stroop and Stroop dilution effects. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[12]  D L Hintzman,et al.  Violations of the independence assumption in process dissociation. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[13]  Michael I. Posner,et al.  Is Word Recognition Automatic? A Cognitive-Anatomical Approach , 1989, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  A Heathcote,et al.  Response time distributions and the Stroop Task: a test of the Cohen, Dunbar, and McClelland (1990) model. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  W. Hockley Analysis of response time distributions in the study of cognitive processes. , 1984 .

[16]  J. Jonides,et al.  On the Cost and Benefit of Cost and Benefit , 1984 .

[17]  A P Yonelinas,et al.  Dissociating automatic and controlled processes in a memory-search task: Beyond implicit memory , 1995, Psychological research.

[18]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A parallel distributed processing approach to automaticity. , 1992, The American journal of psychology.

[19]  B. Dosher,et al.  Serial position and set size in short-term memory: The time course of recognition , 1989 .

[20]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .

[21]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .

[22]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Retrieval Processes in Recognition Memory , 1976 .

[23]  D W Massaro,et al.  Continuous versus discrete information processing in pattern recognition. , 1995, Acta psychologica.

[24]  C M Herdman,et al.  Attentional resource demands of visual word recognition in naming and lexical decisions. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Discrete and continuous models of human information processing: theoretical distinctions and empirical results. , 1988, Acta psychologica.

[26]  Larry L. Jacoby,et al.  In defense of functional independence: Violations of assumptions underlying the process-dissociation procedure? , 1997 .

[27]  L. Jacoby A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory , 1991 .

[28]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[29]  Greg B. Simpson,et al.  Lexical ambiguity and the timecourse of attentional allocation in word recognition. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  R. Ollman Fast guesses in choice reaction time , 1966 .

[31]  G. Logan,et al.  On the Use of a Concurrent Memory Load to Measure Attention and Automaticity , 1979 .

[32]  D A Taylor,et al.  Stage analysis of reaction time. , 1976, Psychological bulletin.

[33]  J P Pieters Sternberg's additive factor method and underlying psychological processes: some theoretical considerations. , 1983, Psychological bulletin.

[34]  Alan S. Brown,et al.  Information Processing and Cognition: The Loyola Symposium , 1976 .

[35]  M. J. Billington,et al.  The Deadline model for simple reaction times , 1972 .

[36]  Philip M. Merikle,et al.  Independence or redundancy? Two models of conscious and unconscious influences , 1993 .

[37]  G. Logan Attention and automaticity in Stroop and priming tasks: Theory and data , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[38]  R. Shiffrin,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: I , 1977 .

[39]  G. Logan Automaticity and cognitive control. , 1989 .

[40]  R. Duncan Luce,et al.  Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization , 1986 .

[41]  C. A. Becker Allocation of attention during visual word recognition. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[42]  Jo P. Pieters,et al.  Sternberg's Additive Factor Method and Underlying Psychological Processes: Some Theoretical Considerations , 1983 .

[43]  E. Erdfelder,et al.  Toward unbiased measurement of conscious and unconscious memory processes within the process dissociation framework. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[44]  D. Strayer,et al.  Attentional Requirements of Automatic and Controlled Processing , 1990 .

[45]  L. Jacoby,et al.  Becoming famous without being recognized: Unconscious influences of memory produced by dividing attention , 1989 .

[46]  D. Mewhort,et al.  Analysis of Response Time Distributions: An Example Using the Stroop Task , 1991 .

[47]  James L. McClelland On the time relations of mental processes: An examination of systems of processes in cascade. , 1979 .

[48]  C W Eriksen,et al.  Information processing in visual search: A continuous flow conception and experimental results , 1979, Perception & psychophysics.

[49]  G. Logan Shapes of reaction-time distributions and shapes of learning curves: a test of the instance theory of automaticity. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[50]  R Ratcliff,et al.  Continuous versus discrete information processing modeling accumulation of partial information. , 1988, Psychological review.

[51]  A. Jacobs,et al.  Models of visual word recognition: Sampling the state of the art. , 1994 .

[52]  T. Carr,et al.  Automaticity in skill acquisition: Mechanisms for reducing interference in concurrent performance. , 1989 .

[53]  H. P. Bahrick,et al.  Time sharing as an index of automatization. , 1958, Journal of experimental psychology.

[54]  James T. Townsend,et al.  The Stochastic Modeling of Elementary Psychological Processes , 1983 .

[55]  Andrew P. Yonelinas,et al.  Separating conscious and unconscious influences of memory: measuring recollection , 1993 .

[56]  F. Donders On the speed of mental processes. , 1969, Acta psychologica.

[57]  Henry L. Roediger,et al.  The Problem of Differing False-Alarm Rates for the Process Dissociation Procedure: Comment on Verfaellie and Treadwell (1993) , 1994 .

[58]  David E. Irwin,et al.  The dynamics of cognition and action: mental processes inferred from speed-accuracy decomposition. , 1988, Psychological review.

[59]  E. Tulving How many memory systems are there , 1985 .

[60]  D. L. Hintzman,et al.  Consequences and causes of correlations in process dissociation. , 1997 .

[61]  H. Egeth,et al.  Capacity limitations in visual word processing. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[62]  J. Lappin,et al.  The latency operating characteristic. I. Effects of stimulus probability on choice reaction time. , 1972, Journal of experimental psychology.

[63]  Saul Sternberg,et al.  The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method , 1969 .

[64]  L. Jacoby,et al.  Stroop process dissociations: the relationship between facilitation and interference. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[65]  John Morton,et al.  Selective Attention to Words and Colours , 1973 .

[66]  J. M. Cattell THE TIME IT TAKES TO SEE AND NAME OBJECTS , 1886 .

[67]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Models of Visual Word RecognitionSampling the State of the Art , 1994 .

[68]  K E Stanovich,et al.  Studies of letter identification using qualitative error analysis: effects of speed stress, tachistoscopic presentation, and word context. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[69]  A. D. Fisk,et al.  Degree of consistent training: Improvements in search performance and automatic process development , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.