The Influence of Implicit Hand-Based Representations on Mental Arithmetic

Recently, a strong functional relationship between finger counting and number processing has been suggested. It has been argued that bodily experiences such as finger counting may influence the structure of the basic mental representations of numbers even in adults. However, to date it remains unclear whether the structure of finger counting systems also influences educated adults’ performance in mental arithmetic. In the present study, we pursued this question by examining finger-based sub-base-five effects in an addition production task. With the standard effect of a carry operation (i.e., base-10 crossing) being replicated, we observed an additional sub-base-five effect such that crossing a sub-base-five boundary led to a relative response time increase. For the case of mental arithmetic sub-base-five effects have previously been reported only in children. However, it remains unclear whether finger-based numerical effects in mental arithmetic reflect an important but transitory step in the development of arithmetical skills. The current findings suggest that even in adults embodied representations such as finger counting patterns modulate arithmetic performance. Thus, they support the general idea that even seemingly abstract cognition in adults may at least partly be rooted in our bodily experiences.

[1]  Ian Thompson,et al.  Mental Calculation Strategies for Addition and Subtraction , 1999 .

[2]  Guilherme Wood,et al.  A developmental fMRI study of nonsymbolic numerical and spatial processing , 2008, Cortex.

[3]  Jamie I. D. Campbell Handbook of mathematical cognition , 2004 .

[4]  Wim Fias,et al.  Passive Hand Movements Disrupt Adults’ Counting Strategies , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[5]  Ardi Roelofs,et al.  Incrementality in naming and reading complex numerals: evidence from eyetracking , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[6]  G. Aschersleben,et al.  The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[7]  Michael Andres,et al.  Contribution of Hand Motor Circuits to Counting , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[8]  Frank Domahs,et al.  Categorical and continuous - disentangling the neural correlates of the carry effect in multi-digit addition , 2010, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[9]  Jamie I. D. Campbell,et al.  Interactive effects of numerical surface form and operand parity in cognitive arithmetic. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[10]  Wim Fias,et al.  The addition of two-digit numbers: exploring carry versus no-carry problems , 2005 .

[11]  M. Zorzi,et al.  Storage and retrieval of addition facts: The role of number comparison , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[12]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Seeing, acting, understanding: motor resonance in language comprehension. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[13]  Pierre Barrouillet,et al.  Predicting arithmetical achievement from neuro-psychological performance: a longitudinal study , 1998, Cognition.

[14]  Frank Domahs,et al.  Embodied numerosity: Implicit hand-based representations influence symbolic number processing across cultures , 2010, Cognition.

[15]  Oliver Lindemann,et al.  Finger Counting Habits in Middle Eastern and Western Individuals: An Online Survey , 2011 .

[16]  Margaret Wilson,et al.  Six views of embodied cognition , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[17]  Mark H. Ashcraft,et al.  Cognitive Psychology and Simple Arithmetic: A Review and Summary of New Directions. , 1995 .

[18]  Etienne Olivier,et al.  Response–effect compatibility of finger–numeral configurations in arithmetical context , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[19]  S. Holm A Simple Sequentially Rejective Multiple Test Procedure , 1979 .

[20]  Vittorio Gallese,et al.  Listening to Action-related Sentences Activates Fronto-parietal Motor Circuits , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[21]  Frank Domahs,et al.  What makes multiplication facts difficult. Problem size or neighborhood consistency? , 2006, Experimental psychology.

[22]  André Vandierendonck,et al.  The role of working memory in the carry operation of mental arithmetic: Number and value of the carry , 2007, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[23]  Ken Nakayama,et al.  Numeric comparison in a visually-guided manual reaching task , 2008, Cognition.

[24]  Michael Andres,et al.  Actions, Words, and Numbers , 2008 .

[25]  P Cormier,et al.  A componential model for mental addition. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[26]  M. Noël,et al.  Does finger training increase young children's numerical performance? , 2008, Cortex.

[27]  Frank Domahs,et al.  Multimodal Semantic Quantity Representations: Further Evidence from Korean Sign Language , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[28]  Michael Andres,et al.  Number magnitude potentiates action judgements , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  M. Noël,et al.  Finger gnosia: a predictor of numerical abilities in children? , 2005, Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence.

[30]  Mark H. Ashcraft,et al.  Evidence for retrieval and decision processes in mental addition , 1978 .

[31]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  What everyone finds: The problem-size effect. , 2005 .

[32]  R. Siegler,et al.  Four aspects of strategic change: contributions to children's learning of multiplication. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[33]  S. Göbel,et al.  Finger Usage and Arithmetic in Adults with Math Difficulties: Evidence From a Case Report , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[34]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  The Mathematical Brain , 1999 .

[35]  Frank Domahs,et al.  Mind the gap between both hands: Evidence for internal finger-based number representations in children's mental calculation , 2008, Cortex.

[36]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Topographical Layout of Hand, Eye, Calculation, and Language-Related Areas in the Human Parietal Lobe , 2002, Neuron.

[37]  Michael Andres,et al.  Number magnitude and grip aperture interaction , 2004, Neuroreport.

[38]  Matthias Schlesewsky,et al.  Behavioral and Brain Functions , 2008 .

[39]  G. Groen,et al.  A chronometric analysis of simple addition. , 1972 .

[40]  S. Dehaene Symbols and quantities in parietal cortex: elements of a mathematical theory of number representation and manipulation , 2007 .

[41]  I. Johnsrude,et al.  Somatotopic Representation of Action Words in Human Motor and Premotor Cortex , 2004, Neuron.

[42]  A. Vandierendonck,et al.  The odd-even effect in addition: an analysis per problem type. , 2005, Experimental psychology.

[43]  Michael P. Kaschak,et al.  Grounding language in action , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[44]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Dexterity with numbers: rTMS over left angular gyrus disrupts finger gnosis and number processing , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[45]  Jian Kong,et al.  The neural substrate of arithmetic operations and procedure complexity. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[46]  Frank Domahs,et al.  To carry or not to carry--is this the question? Disentangling the carry effect in multi-digit addition. , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[47]  Martin H. Fischer,et al.  Finger counting habits modulate spatial-numerical associations , 2008, Cortex.

[48]  Ulrike Cress,et al.  Sensori-motor spatial training of number magnitude representation , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.