Attentional Biases during Steering Behavior

In the current study, we examine eye movements of human operators during a combined steering and discrimination task. In this task, observers had to alternate their gaze between a central steering task and a discrimination task in the periphery. Our results show that the observer's gaze behavior is influenced by the motion direction of the steering task. Saccade reaction times (SRTs) of saccades to the discrimination target were shorter if the target appeared in the steering direction. SRTs back to the steering task were shorter when the steering target moved away from the discrimination target. These effects are likely the result of motion-related attention shifts and an interaction of the saccadic and smooth pursuit eye movement system.

[1]  J. Findlay,et al.  Active Vision: The Psychology of Looking and Seeing , 2003 .

[2]  Heiner Deubel,et al.  The mind's eye : cognitive and applied aspects of eye movement research , 2003 .

[3]  Robert W. Proctor,et al.  Human information processing: an overview for human-computer interaction , 2002 .

[4]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  The relationship between exogenous and endogenous saccades and attention. , 2003 .

[5]  M. Tanaka,et al.  Latency of saccades during smooth-pursuit eye movement in man Directional asymmetries , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  Ronald A. Hess Pursuit Tracking and Higher Levels of Skill Development in the Human Pilot , 1981, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics.

[7]  Jason Bell,et al.  Local motion effects on form in radial frequency patterns. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[8]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[9]  Thom Baguley,et al.  Calculating and graphing within-subject confidence intervals for ANOVA , 2012, Behavior research methods.

[10]  Jillian H. Fecteau,et al.  Salience, relevance, and firing: a priority map for target selection , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[11]  Yongjin Kwon,et al.  Evaluation of synthetic vision information system (SVIS) displays based on pilot performance , 2001, 20th DASC. 20th Digital Avionics Systems Conference (Cat. No.01CH37219).

[12]  Ulrich Büttner,et al.  Saccades to stationary and moving targets differ in the monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  Kevin A. Gluck,et al.  Cognitive Control in a Computational Model of the Predator Pilot , 2007, Integrated Models of Cognitive Systems.

[14]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Retinal eccentricity and the latency of eye saccades , 1994, Vision Research.

[15]  H. Nothdurft The role of features in preattentive vision: Comparison of orientation, motion and color cues , 1993, Vision Research.

[16]  Gunnar Blohm,et al.  The default allocation of attention is broadly ahead of smooth pursuit. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[17]  田中真樹 Latency of Saccades during Smooth Pursuit Eye Movement in Man: Directional asymmetries(ヒト滑動性眼球運動の最中の視覚誘導性サッカードの潜時変化) , 1998 .

[18]  Frank E. Ritter,et al.  The Rise of Cognitive Architectures , 2007, Integrated Models of Cognitive Systems.

[19]  M. Missal,et al.  What triggers catch-up saccades during visual tracking? , 2002, Journal of Neurophysiology.

[20]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Stimulus-driven capture and contingent capture. , 2010, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[22]  R. Gellman,et al.  Motion processing for saccadic eye movements in humans , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  B. Dosher,et al.  The role of attention in the programming of saccades , 1995, Vision Research.

[24]  Andrew Sears and Julie A. Jacko The human-computer interaction handbook , 2013 .

[25]  G. Stricker American Psychological Association Code of Ethics , 2010 .