Newborns’ preference for goal-directed actions

The central role of sensory-motor representations in cognitive functions is almost universally accepted. However, determining the link between motor execution and its sensory counterpart and when, during ontogenesis, this link originates are still under investigation. The aim of the present study is to investigate whether at birth this link is already present and 2-day-old newborns are able to discriminate between visual cues indicating goal-directed or non-goal-directed actions. Here, with a preferential looking technique, a hand grasping a ball was the observed movement and we orthogonally manipulated the three factors necessary to successfully reach the goal: (a) presence of the ball, (b) direction of the arm movement, and (c) hand shaping. Results indicated that newborns orient more frequently and look longer at a hand shape for whole hand prehension but only when the movement is directed away from the body and toward the external world. In addition, newborns prefer the away from the body movement only when the object is present. We argue that newborns prefer a movement directed toward the external world only when it may develop into a purposeful movement because of the presence of the to-be-grasped object. Overall, our results support the existence of primitive sensory-motor associations since the first days after birth.

[1]  J. W. Sparling,et al.  Quantitative Measurement of Fetal Movement , 1993 .

[2]  C. Hofsten Developmental changes in the organization of prereaching movements , 1984 .

[3]  U. Castiello,et al.  Evidence of early development of action planning in the human foetus: a kinematic study , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  A. Streri,et al.  Cross-modal recognition of shape from hand to eyes and handedness in human newborns , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  Jessica A. Sommerville,et al.  Pulling out the intentional structure of action: the relation between action processing and action production in infancy , 2005, Cognition.

[6]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[7]  V. Gallese,et al.  Wired to Be Social: The Ontogeny of Human Interaction , 2010, PloS one.

[8]  C. Hofsten,et al.  Infants predict other people's action goals , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  F. Simion,et al.  A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn baby , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[10]  P. Hauf Infants' perception and production of intentional actions. , 2007, Progress in brain research.

[11]  J. Sommerville,et al.  Action experience alters 3-month-old infants' perception of others' actions , 2005, Cognition.

[12]  M. Jeannerod The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[13]  C. Von Hofsten Action, the foundation for cognitive development. , 2009, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[14]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The Cortical Motor System , 2001, Neuron.

[15]  A. Meer Keeping the arm in the limelight: advanced visual control of arm movements in neonates. , 1996 .

[16]  G. Butterworth,et al.  The Effects of Hunger on Hand-Mouth Coordination in Newborn Infants. , 1995 .

[17]  A. Streri,et al.  Cross-modal recognition of shape from hand to eyes in human newborns , 2003, Somatosensory & motor research.

[18]  R. Johansson,et al.  Action plans used in action observation , 2003, Nature.

[19]  J. W. Sparling,et al.  Fetal and neonatal hand movement. , 1999, Physical therapy.

[20]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Eye contact detection in humans from birth , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[21]  Max Coltheart,et al.  Cognitive Neuropsychology , 2014, Scholarpedia.