Motor task difficulty and brain activity: investigation of goal-directed reciprocal aiming using positron emission tomography.

Differences in the kinematics and pattern of relative regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) during goal-directed arm aiming were investigated with the use of a Fitts continuous aiming paradigm with three difficulty conditions (index of difficulty, ID) and two aiming types (transport vs. targeting) in six healthy right-handed young participants with the use of video-based movement trajectory analysis and positron emission tomography. Movement time and kinematic characteristics were analyzed together with the magnitude of cerebral blood flow to identify areas of brain activity proportionate to task and movement variables. Significant differences in rCBF between task conditions were determined by analysis of variance with planned comparisons of means with the use of group mean weighted linear contrasts. Data were first analyzed for the group. Then individual subject differences for the movement versus no movement and task difficulty comparisons were related to each individual subjects' anatomy by magnetic resonance imaging. Significant differences in rCBF during reciprocal aiming compared with no-movement conditions were found in a mosaic of well-known cortical and subcortical areas associated with the planning and execution of goal-directed movements. These included cortical areas in the left sensorimotor, dorsal premotor, and ventral premotor cortices, caudal supplementary motor area (SMA) proper, and parietal cortex, and subcortical areas in the left putamen, globus pallidus, red nucleus, thalamus, and anterior cerebellum. As aiming task difficulty (ID) increased, rCBF increased in areas associated with the planning of more complex movements requiring greater visuomotor processing. These included bilateral occipital, left inferior parietal, and left dorsal cingulate cortices--caudal SMA proper and right dorsal premotor area. These same areas showed significant increases or decreases, respectively, when contrast means were compared with the use of movement time or relative acceleration time, respectively, as the weighting factor. Analysis of individual subject differences revealed a correspondence between the spatial extent of rCBF changes as a function of task ID and the individuals' movement times. As task ID decreased, significant increases in rCBF were evident in the right anterior cerebellum, left middle occipital gyrus, and right ventral premotor area. Functionally, these areas are associated with aiming conditions in which the motor execution demands are high (i.e., coordination of rapid reversals) and precise trajectory planning is minimal. These same areas showed significant increases or decreases, respectively, when contrast means were compared with the use of movement time or relative acceleration time, respectively, as the weighting factor. A functional dissociation resulted from the weighted linear contrasts between larger (limb transport) or smaller (endpoint targeting) type amplitude/target width aiming conditions. Areas with significantly greater rCBF for targeting were the left motor cortex, left intraparietal sulcus, and left caudate. In contrast, those areas with greater rCBF associated with limb transport included bilateral occipital lingual gyri and the right anterior cerebellum. Various theoretical explanations for the speed/accuracy tradeoffs of rapid aiming movements have been proposed since the original information theory hypothesis of Fitts. This is the first report to relate the predictable variations in motor control under changing task constraints with the functional anatomy of these rapid goal-directed aiming movements. Differences in unimanual aiming task difficulty lead to dissociable activation of cortical-subcortical networks. Further, these data suggest that when more precise targeting is required, independent of task difficulty, a cortical-subcortical loop composed of the contralateral motor cortex, intraparietal sulcus, and caudate is activated. This is consistent with the role of motor cortex

[1]  Robert Sessions Woodworth,et al.  THE ACCURACY OF VOLUNTARY MOVEMENT , 1899 .

[2]  Stern Bt,et al.  EXCERPTA MEDICA. , 1974, Canadian Medical Association journal.

[3]  P. Fitts The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  P. Fitts,et al.  INFORMATION CAPACITY OF DISCRETE MOTOR RESPONSES. , 1964, Journal of experimental psychology.

[5]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  V. Brooks,et al.  Effects of cooling dentate nucleus on tracking-task performance in monkeys. , 1973, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  Michael H. Kutner Applied Linear Statistical Models , 1974 .

[8]  H. Zelaznik,et al.  Motor-output variability: a theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. , 1979, Psychological review.

[9]  B. Larsen,et al.  Activation of the supplementary motor area during voluntary movement in man suggests it works as a supramotor area. , 1979, Science.

[10]  P. Roland,et al.  Regional cerebral blood flow changes in cortex and basal ganglia during voluntary movements in normal human volunteers. , 1982, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  M. D. Crutcher,et al.  Relations between parameters of step-tracking movements and single cell discharge in the globus pallidus and subthalamic nucleus of the behaving monkey , 1983, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[12]  E. R. Crossman,et al.  Feedback Control of Hand-Movement and Fitts' Law , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[13]  S. A. Wallace,et al.  Visual Control of Discrete Aiming Movements , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[14]  M. Raichle,et al.  Brain blood flow measured with intravenous H2(15)O. I. Theory and error analysis. , 1983, Journal of nuclear medicine : official publication, Society of Nuclear Medicine.

[15]  M. Mintun,et al.  Brain blood flow measured with intravenous H2(15)O. II. Implementation and validation. , 1983, Journal of nuclear medicine : official publication, Society of Nuclear Medicine.

[16]  J. Brodzinski Three-way analysis of variance , 1983 .

[17]  M. Mintun,et al.  A Noninvasive Approach to Quantitative Functional Brain Mapping with H215O and Positron Emission Tomography , 1984, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[18]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  A Noninvasive Positron Computed Tomography Technique Using Oxygen-15-Labeled Water for the Evaluation of Neurobehavioral Task Batteries , 1985, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[19]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Neuronal population coding of movement direction. , 1986, Science.

[20]  C. MacKenzie,et al.  Three-Dimensional Movement Trajectories in Fitts' Task: Implications for Control , 1987 .

[21]  Ronald F. Zernicke,et al.  Predictions for neural control based on limb dynamics , 1987, Trends in Neurosciences.

[22]  Daniel Bullock,et al.  Neural dynamics of planned arm movements: emergent invariants and speed-accuracy properties during trajectory formation , 1988 .

[23]  R A Abrams,et al.  Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. , 1988, Psychological review.

[24]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Primate motor cortex and free arm movements to visual targets in three- dimensional space. I. Relations between single cell discharge and direction of movement , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[25]  S. Grossberg,et al.  Neural dynamics of planned arm movements: emergent invariants and speed-accuracy properties during trajectory formation. , 1988, Psychological review.

[26]  R. Schmidt,et al.  Changes in limb dynamics during the practice of rapid arm movements. , 1989, Journal of biomechanics.

[27]  A. Riehle,et al.  Monkey primary motor and premotor cortex: single-cell activity related to prior information about direction and extent of an intended movement. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Basal ganglia-thalamocortical circuits: parallel substrates for motor, oculomotor, "prefrontal" and "limbic" functions. , 1990, Progress in brain research.

[29]  T. Milner,et al.  The effect of accuracy constraints on three-dimensional movement kinematics , 1990, Neuroscience.

[30]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Comparing Functional (PET) Images: The Assessment of Significant Change , 1991, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[31]  L. Jakobson,et al.  A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them , 1991, Nature.

[32]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  A Three-Dimensional Statistical Analysis for CBF Activation Studies in Human Brain , 1992, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[33]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Human functional anatomy of visually guided finger movements. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[34]  J. Kelso,et al.  Theoretical concepts and strategies for understanding perceptual-motor skill: from information capacity in closed systems to self-organization in open, nonequilibrium systems. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[35]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Rapid Automated Algorithm for Aligning and Reslicing PET Images , 1992, Journal of computer assisted tomography.

[36]  上村 和夫,et al.  Quantification of brain function : tracer kinetics and image analysis in brain PET : proceedings of Brain PET '93 Akita : Quantification of Brain Function, Akita, Japan, 29-31 May, 1993 , 1993 .

[37]  M. Honda,et al.  Both primary motor cortex and supplementary motor area play an important role in complex finger movement. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[38]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Automated image registration , 1993 .

[39]  T. Ebner,et al.  Neuronal specification of direction and distance during reaching movements in the superior precentral premotor area and primary motor cortex of monkeys. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  MRI‐PET Registration with Automated Algorithm , 1993, Journal of computer assisted tomography.

[41]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Movement parameters and neural activity in motor cortex and area 5. , 1994, Cerebral cortex.

[42]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Parceling of mesial frontal motor areas during ideation and movement using functional magnetic resonance imaging at 1.5 tesla , 1994, Annals of neurology.

[43]  J. Tanji The supplementary motor area in the cerebral cortex , 1994, Neuroscience Research.

[44]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Functional imaging of procedural motor learning: Relating cerebral blood flow with individual subject performance , 1994, Human brain mapping.

[45]  M. Jeannerod The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[46]  Y. Samson,et al.  Movement‐ and task‐related activations of motor cortical areas: A positron emission tomographic study , 1994, Annals of neurology.

[47]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Mapping motor representations with positron emission tomography , 1994, Nature.

[48]  Remo Guidieri Res , 1995, RES: Anthropology and Aesthetics.

[49]  Réjean Plamondon,et al.  A kinematic theory of rapid human movements , 1995, Biological Cybernetics.

[50]  A. Georgopoulos Current issues in directional motor control , 1995, Trends in Neurosciences.

[51]  R. Passingham,et al.  Functional anatomy of the mental representation of upper extremity movements in healthy subjects. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[52]  Réjean Plamondon,et al.  A kinematic theory of rapid human movements , 1995, Biological Cybernetics.

[53]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Cerebellar ataxia: abnormal control of interaction torques across multiple joints. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[54]  P. Strick,et al.  Motor areas of the medial wall: a review of their location and functional activation. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[55]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Functional anatomy of pointing and grasping in humans. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[56]  C. Winstein,et al.  The locus of age-related movement slowing: sensory processing in continuous goal-directed aiming. , 1996, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[57]  R. Myers Quantification of brain function using PET , 1996 .