An Exemplar Model For Perceptual Categorization Of Events

Publisher Summary This chapter proposes a representation scheme that covers both static objects and events. It also specifies the mechanisms that support classification of represented events. The main concern of this chapter is the categorization of events. Although objects can be represented in many different ways, it seems sensible to start from a very general scheme that proves its value in numerous studies of perceptual classification of static objects. To represent events, a framework can be adapted in two different ways. The first possibility maintains the assumption that objects correspond to single points in a multidimensional space. The second approach abandons the assumption that objects must correspond to single points in the psychological space. Perceptual categorization is the process of assigning a category label to a perceived object. It is an essential component of numerous cognitive tasks, including object naming and recognition, perceptual learning, inductive and deductive reasoning about objects, and so forth. It is important to make a clear distinction between categorization of dynamic objects and categorization of events. Events are episodes in the existence of objects, with well-defined temporal boundaries. Events can also involve static objects, and the same dynamic object can occur in different events.

[1]  M. Arterberry,et al.  Three-month-old infants' categorization of animals and vehicles based on static and dynamic attributes. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[2]  Koen Lamberts,et al.  The time course of categorization. , 1998 .

[3]  E S Spelke,et al.  Infants' sensitivity to effects of gravity on visible object motion. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  F. Pollick,et al.  Exaggerating Temporal Differences Enhances Recognition of Individuals from Point Light Displays , 2000, Psychological science.

[5]  J S Lappin,et al.  On the relation between time and space in the visual discrimination of velocity. , 1975, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  Joann M. Montepare,et al.  A cross-cultural comparison of impressions created by age-related variations in gait , 1993 .

[7]  W T Maddox,et al.  Comparing decision bound and exemplar models of categorization , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[8]  S. Runeson,et al.  Constant velocity — Not perceived as such , 1974, Psychological research.

[9]  Estes Wk The problem of inference from curves based on group data. , 1956 .

[10]  S. Lea,et al.  Perception of Emotion from Dynamic Point-Light Displays Represented in Dance , 1996, Perception.

[11]  M. Bertamini,et al.  Understanding projectile acceleration. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[12]  Douglas L. Medin,et al.  Context theory of classification learning. , 1978 .

[13]  Mark A. Pitt,et al.  Toward a method of selecting among computational models of cognition. , 2002 .

[14]  R. Duncan Luce,et al.  Several unresolved conceptual problems of mathematical psychology , 1997 .

[15]  Robert F. Port,et al.  Naive time, temporal patterns, and human audition , 1996 .

[16]  Takeo Watanabe,et al.  High-Level Motion Processing , 1998 .

[17]  J. Kruschke,et al.  ALCOVE: an exemplar-based connectionist model of category learning. , 1992, Psychological review.

[18]  R. Nosofsky Attention, similarity, and the identification-categorization relationship. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[19]  William K. Estes,et al.  Classification and cognition , 1994 .

[20]  R. Nosofsky Attention and learning processes in the identification and categorization of integral stimuli. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[21]  I. J. Myung,et al.  Toward an explanation of the power law artifact: Insights from response surface analysis , 2000, Memory & cognition.

[22]  Geoffrey P. Bingham,et al.  Dynamics and the problem of visual event recognition , 1996 .

[23]  W T Maddox,et al.  On the dangers of averaging across observers when comparing decision bound models and generalized context models of categorization , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[24]  G. Mather,et al.  Gender discrimination in biological motion displays based on dynamic cues , 1994, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[25]  Benise S. K Mak,et al.  The role of motion in children's categorization of objects , 1999, Cognition.

[26]  Thomas J. Palmeri,et al.  An Exemplar-Based Random Walk Model of Speeded Classification , 1997 .

[27]  R. Nosofsky Similarity Scaling and Cognitive Process Models , 1992 .

[28]  Geoffrey P Bingham,et al.  Trajectory forms as a source of information about events , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.

[29]  Evan Heit,et al.  Perceptual processes in matching and recognition of complex pictures. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  K. Lamberts Information-accumulation theory of speeded categorization. , 2000, Psychological review.

[31]  Geoffrey P. Bingham,et al.  The use of time and trajectory forms as visual information about spatial scale in events , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.

[32]  Michael McCloskey,et al.  Naive physics: the curvilinear impetus principle and its role in interactions with moving objects , 1983 .

[33]  G. Johansson Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .

[34]  S. Brownlow,et al.  Perception of movement and dancer characteristics from point-light displays of dance , 1997 .

[35]  G P Bingham,et al.  Infant sensitivity to trajectory forms. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[36]  G P Bingham,et al.  Dynamics and the orientation of kinematic forms in visual event recognition. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  John E. Opfer,et al.  Identifying living and sentient kinds from dynamic information: the case of goal-directed versus aimless autonomous movement in conceptual change , 2002, Cognition.

[38]  Koen Lamberts,et al.  Categorization under time pressure. , 1995 .

[39]  Sung-Bae Cho,et al.  Estimating the efficiency of recognizing gender and affect from biological motion , 2002, Vision Research.

[40]  G. Orban,et al.  Factors influencing velocity coding in the human visual system , 1984, Vision Research.

[41]  D. Rosenbaum Perception and extrapolation of velocity and acceleration. , 1975 .

[42]  S C McKinley,et al.  Investigations of exemplar and decision bound models in large, ill-defined category structures. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[43]  Andrew T. Smith,et al.  Visual detection of motion , 1994 .

[44]  R. Fox,et al.  The perception of biological motion by human infants. , 1982, Science.

[45]  Eugene Galanter,et al.  Handbook of mathematical psychology: I. , 1963 .

[46]  Armin Bruderlin,et al.  Perceiving affect from arm movement , 2001, Cognition.

[47]  R. Blake Cats Perceive Biological Motion , 1993 .

[48]  R. M. Siegel,et al.  Infant use of relative motion as information for form: Evidence for spatiotemporal integration of complex motion displays , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[49]  Martha E Arterberry,et al.  Infant perceptual and conceptual categorization: the roles of static and dynamic stimulus attributes , 2002, Cognition.

[50]  G. Bingham,et al.  Kinematic form and scaling: further investigations on the visual perception of lifted weight. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.