Wanted: Fully Operational Definitions of Dissociations in Single-Case Studies

In contrast to the careful consideration given to the issue of what we can infer from dissociations in single-case studies, the more basic question of how we decide whether a dissociation is present has been relatively neglected. Proposals are made for fully operational definitions of a deficit, classical and strong dissociations, and double dissociations. In developing these definitions it was assumed that they should be based on the use of inferential rather than descriptive statistical methods. The scope of these definitions is limited to typical single-case studies in which patients are compared to control samples of a modest size. The operational definition of a classical dissociation incorporates a requirement that a patient's performance on Task X is significantly different from Task Y, in addition to the "standard" requirement that the patient has a deficit on Task X and is within normal limits on Task Y. We ran a simulation to estimate the Type I error rates when the criteria for dissociations are applied and found these to be low (Type I errors were defined as identifying an individual from the control population as having a dissociation). The inferential methods for testing whether the various criteria are met make use of t-distributions. These methods are contrasted with the widespread use of z to test for a deficit or a difference between tasks. In the latter approach the statistics of the control sample are treated as parameters; this is not appropriate when, as is normally the case, the control sample size is modest in size.

[1]  R. Linn Educational measurement, 3rd ed. , 1989 .

[2]  K Willmes,et al.  An approach to analyzing a single subject's scores obtained in a standardized test with application to the Aachen Aphasia Test (AAT). , 1985, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[3]  T. Shallice From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure , 1988 .

[4]  David J. Hand,et al.  Elements of Statistics , 1996 .

[5]  John C. Marshall,et al.  Idealisation meets psychometrics: The case for the right groups and the right individuals , 1988 .

[6]  A. Caramazza,et al.  The case for single-patient studies , 1988 .

[7]  E. Renzi,et al.  Apperceptive and Associative Forms of Prosopagnosia , 1991, Cortex.

[8]  P. Garthwaite,et al.  Inferential methods for comparing a single case with a control sample: modified t‐tests versus mycroft et al.'s (2002) modified anova , 2004, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[9]  R. Payne,et al.  Statistics for the investigation of individual cases. , 1957, Journal of clinical psychology.

[10]  J R Crawford,et al.  Payne and Jones revisited: estimating the abnormality of test score differences using a modified paired samples t test. , 1998, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[11]  D. C. Howell,et al.  Comparing an Individual's Test Score Against Norms Derived from Small Samples , 1998 .

[12]  Janice Kay,et al.  An evaluation of statistical procedures for comparing an individual's performance with that of a group of controls , 2002, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[13]  P. Garthwaite,et al.  Investigation of the single case in neuropsychology: confidence limits on the abnormality of test scores and test score differences , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  Kim Kirsner,et al.  What Can we Infer from Double Dissociations? , 2003, Cortex.

[15]  John R. Crawford,et al.  Psychometric Foundations of Neuropsychological Assessment , 2005 .

[16]  Laura H. Goldstein,et al.  Clinical neuropsychology: a practical guide to assessment and management for clinicians , 2005 .

[17]  Erminio Capitani,et al.  Normative Data and Neuropsychological Assessment. Common Problems in Clinical Practice and Research , 1997 .

[18]  M. Coltheart Assumptions and methods in cognitive neuropsychology. , 2001 .

[19]  Douglas G. Altman,et al.  Statistics with confidence: Confidence intervals and statistical guidelines . , 1990 .

[20]  Tim Shallice,et al.  Case study approach in neuropsychological research , 1979 .