Cerebral responses to infant-directed speech and the effect of talker familiarity

A number of behavioral studies suggest that infant-directed speech (IDS) plays a more important role in facilitating both: a) speech perception, and b) adult-infant social interactions than does adult-directed speech (ADS), and hence that IDS contributes to subsequent social and language development. However neural substrates that may underlie these IDS functions have not been examined. The present study examined cerebral hemodynamic responses to IDS in 48 infants (4-13 months of age) using near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS). Japanese sentences uttered by the infants' own mothers and by unfamiliar mothers were used to record activations in temporal and frontal area separately. Increased activations were observed predominantly in infants' left and right temporal areas when they listened to IDS rather than to ADS when both involved voices of their own and unfamiliar mothers. In contrast, significantly greater activations were observed in the frontal area when infants listened to IDS produced by their own mothers, not when IDS arose from unfamiliar mothers. Furthermore, the present results indicate that responses to IDS do vary as a function of the infant's age and the talker familiarity. These findings suggest a differential function for frontal and temporal areas in processing infant-directed speech by the different speakers.

[1]  Erik D. Thiessen,et al.  Infant-Directed Speech Facilitates Word Segmentation. , 2005, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[2]  J. Jacobson,et al.  Paralinguistic Features of Adult Speech to Infants and Small Children , 1983 .

[3]  A. Fernald,et al.  Intonation and communicative intent in mothers' speech to infants: is the melody the message? , 1989, Child development.

[4]  R. Aslin,et al.  Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. , 1990, Child development.

[5]  Hiroki Sato,et al.  Effect of fMRI acoustic noise on sensorimotor activation examined using optical topography , 2006, NeuroImage.

[6]  S. Takashima,et al.  Human Visual Cortical Function during Photic Stimulation Monitoring by Means of near-Infrared Spectroscopy , 1993, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[7]  M. Tamura,et al.  Interpretation of near-infrared spectroscopy signals: a study with a newly developed perfused rat brain model. , 2001, Journal of applied physiology.

[8]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Determinants of dominance: Is language laterality explained by physical or linguistic features of speech? , 2005, NeuroImage.

[9]  K. Wheldall,et al.  Pitch alterations in British motherese: some preliminary acoustic data , 1989, Journal of Child Language.

[10]  K. Mori,et al.  Developmental changes in cerebral lateralization during speech processing measured by near infrared spectroscopy , 2003 .

[11]  E. Leibenluft,et al.  Mothers' neural activation in response to pictures of their children and other children , 2004, Biological Psychiatry.

[12]  M. Papoušek,et al.  Tone and intonation in Mandarin babytalk to presyllabic infants: Comparison with registers of adult conversation and foreign language instruction , 1991, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[13]  M. Lowe,et al.  A Cross-Linguistic fMRI Study of Spectral and Temporal Cues Underlying Phonological Processing , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  Jane A. Baran,et al.  Hemispheric specialization in processing intonation contours , 1996 .

[15]  D. Stern,et al.  Intonation contours as signals in maternal speech to prelinguistic infants. , 1982 .

[16]  K. Sakatani,et al.  Auditory-evoked cerebral oxygenation changes in hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy of newborn infants monitored by near infrared spectroscopy. , 2002, Early human development.

[17]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Lateralization of phonetic and pitch discrimination in speech processing. , 1992, Science.

[18]  David A. Boas,et al.  Factors affecting the accuracy of near-infrared spectroscopy concentration calculations for focal changes in oxygenation parameters , 2003, NeuroImage.

[19]  Angela D. Friederici,et al.  The Detection of Communicative Signals Directed at the Self in Infant Prefrontal Cortex , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[20]  J. Bohannon,et al.  Intonation Patterns in Child-Directed Speech: Mother-Father Differences. , 1984 .

[21]  J. Campos,et al.  “Stranger” Distress: Confirmation of a Developmental Shift in a Longitudinal Sample , 1976 .

[22]  Fumitaka Homae,et al.  The right hemisphere of sleeping infant perceives sentential prosody , 2006, Neuroscience Research.

[23]  Eric E. Nelson,et al.  Orbitofrontal cortex tracks positive mood in mothers viewing pictures of their newborn infants , 2004, NeuroImage.

[24]  A. Decasper,et al.  Of Human Bonding: Newborns Prefer Their Mothers' Voices , 1980 .

[25]  Philip A. Gable,et al.  The role of asymmetric frontal cortical activity in emotion-related phenomena: A review and update , 2010, Biological Psychology.

[26]  Patricia K. Kuhl,et al.  Fast Track , 2020, Electric and Hybrid Vehicle Technology International.

[27]  L. Trainor,et al.  Is Infant-Directed Speech Prosody a Result of the Vocal Expression of Emotion? , 2000, Psychological science.

[28]  R. Homan,et al.  Cerebral location of international 10-20 system electrode placement. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[29]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Maternal speech to infants in a tonal language: Support for universal prosodic features in motherese. , 1988 .

[30]  A. Fernald,et al.  Expanded Intonation Contours in Mothers' Speech to Newborns. , 1984 .

[31]  Masako Okamoto,et al.  Three-dimensional probabilistic anatomical cranio-cerebral correlation via the international 10–20 system oriented for transcranial functional brain mapping , 2004, NeuroImage.

[32]  Catherine T. Best,et al.  Left-Hemisphere Advantage for Click Consonants is Determined by Linguistic Significance and Experience , 1999 .

[33]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  A moment of silence: How the prosodic cues in motherese might assist language learning , 1986 .

[34]  G. Taga,et al.  Brain imaging in awake infants by near-infrared optical topography , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[35]  I. Johnsrude,et al.  Spectral and temporal processing in human auditory cortex. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[36]  Rochelle S. Newman,et al.  Children's use of the prosodic characteristics of infant-directed speech , 2003 .

[37]  Johannes Lehtonen,et al.  Cerebral processing of mother's voice compared to unfamiliar voice in 4-month-old infants. , 2004, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[38]  Y. Hoshi,et al.  Hemodynamic responses to photic stimulation in neonates. , 2000, Pediatric neurology.

[39]  J. Werker,et al.  Preference for infant-directed over adult-directed speech: Evidence from 7-week-old infants , 1992 .

[40]  L. Trainor,et al.  Frontal brain electrical activity (EEG) and heart rate in response to affective infant-directed (ID) speech in 9-month-old infants , 2007, Brain and Cognition.

[41]  D. Burnham,et al.  Universality and specificity in infant-directed speech: Pitch modifications as a function of infant age and sex in a tonal and non-tonal language , 2001 .

[42]  Frontal cerebral blood flow change associated with , 2007 .

[43]  R. Aslin,et al.  Developmental differences in infant attention to the spectral properties of infant-directed speech. , 1994, Child development.

[44]  J. Karhu,et al.  Four-month-old infants process own mother's voice faster than unfamiliar voices--electrical signs of sensitization in infant brain. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[45]  David Poeppel,et al.  The analysis of speech in different temporal integration windows: cerebral lateralization as 'asymmetric sampling in time' , 2003, Speech Commun..

[46]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Pragmatics of Sibling Speech to One Year Olds. , 1985 .

[47]  K. Sakatani,et al.  Cerebral blood oxygenation changes induced by auditory stimulation in newborn infants measured by near infrared spectroscopy. , 1999, Early human development.

[48]  Nick Everdell,et al.  Selective Cortical Mapping of Biological Motion Processing in Young Infants , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[49]  R. Spitz The First Year of Life , 1981 .

[50]  D. Stern,et al.  The prosody of maternal speech: infant age and context related changes , 1983, Journal of Child Language.

[51]  C. A. Ferguson,et al.  Talking to Children: Language Input and Acquisition , 1979 .

[52]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Functional Neuroimaging of Speech Perception in Infants , 2002, Science.

[53]  Nobuo Masataka,et al.  Motherese in a signed language , 1992 .

[54]  Karin S. Frey,et al.  Infants of depressed mothers exhibit atypical frontal electrical brain activity during interactions with mother and with a familiar, nondepressed adult. , 1999, Child development.

[55]  David A. Boas,et al.  Differences in the hemodynamic response to event-related motor and visual paradigms as measured by near-infrared spectroscopy , 2003, NeuroImage.

[56]  Renate Zangl,et al.  Increased brain activity to infant-directed speech in 6-and 13-month-old infants. , 2007 .

[57]  J. Werker,et al.  Infant preference for both male and female infant-directed talk: a developmental study of attentional and affective responsiveness. , 1989, Canadian journal of psychology.

[58]  P. Hepper,et al.  Newborn and fetal response to maternal voice , 1993 .

[59]  K. Mori,et al.  [Cerebral lateralization in spoken language processing measured by multi-channel near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS)]. , 2003, No to shinkei = Brain and nerve.

[60]  N. Fox,et al.  Asymmetrical brain activity discriminates between positive and negative affective stimuli in human infants. , 1982, Science.

[61]  Rochelle S. Newman,et al.  Listen to your mother! The role of talker familiarity in infant streaming , 2004, Cognition.

[62]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Language or music, mother or Mozart? Structural and environmental influences on infants’ language networks , 2010, Brain and Language.

[63]  Franck Ramus,et al.  Optical brain imaging reveals general auditory and language-specific processing in early infant development. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[64]  Fumitaka Homae,et al.  Prosodic processing in the developing brain , 2007, Neuroscience Research.

[65]  N. Fox,et al.  Electroencephalogram Asymmetry in Response to the Approach of a Stranger and Maternal Separation in 10-Month-Old Infants. , 1987 .

[66]  Koichi Mori,et al.  Neural Attunement Processes in Infants during the Acquisition of a Language-Specific Phonemic Contrast , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[67]  David A. Boas,et al.  A Quantitative Comparison of Simultaneous BOLD fMRI and NIRS Recordings during Functional Brain Activation , 2002, NeuroImage.

[68]  Angela D. Friederici,et al.  The Developmental Origins of Voice Processing in the Human Brain , 2010, Neuron.

[69]  S Kiritani,et al.  Developmental change in auditory preferences for speech stimuli in Japanese infants. , 2001, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[70]  M. Tamura,et al.  Dynamic multichannel near-infrared optical imaging of human brain activity. , 1993, Journal of applied physiology.

[71]  J. Mehler,et al.  Sounds and silence: An optical topography study of language recognition at birth , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[72]  Hugo Lagercrantz,et al.  Cerebral Hemodynamic Response to Unpleasant Odors in the Preterm Newborn Measured by Near-Infrared Spectroscopy , 2001, Pediatric Research.

[73]  Ippeita Dan,et al.  Prefrontal activation associated with social attachment: facial-emotion recognition in mothers and infants. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.