The Neural Basis and Evolution of Divergent and Convergent Thought

This chapter takes as its departure point a neural level theory of insight that arose from studies of the sparse, distributed, content-addressable architecture of associative memory. It is argued that convergent thought is most fruitfully characterized in terms of, not the generation of a single correct solution (as it is conventionally construed), but using concepts in their most compact form by activating neural cell assemblies that respond to their most typical properties. This allows them to be deployed in a conventional manner such that effort is reserved for exploring causal relationships. Conversely, it is argued that divergent thought is most fruitfully characterized in terms of, not the generation of multiple solutions (as it is conventionally construed), but using concepts in a form that is, albeit expanded, constrained by the situation, by activating neural cell assemblies that respond to context-specific atypical properties. This allows them to be deployed in a manner that is conducive to exploring unconventional yet potentially relevant associations, and unearthing potentially useful relationships of correlation. Thus, divergent thought can involve as few as one idea. This proposal is compatible with widespread beliefs that (1) most creative tasks require not many solutions but one, yet entail both divergent and convergent thinking, and (2) not all problems with multiple solutions require creative thinking, and conversely, some problems with single solution do require creative thought. The chapter discusses how the ability to shift between convergent and divergent modes of thought may have evolved, and it concludes with educational and vocational implications.

[1]  Paul T. Sowden,et al.  The shifting sands of creative thinking: Connections to dual-process theory , 2014, 1409.2207.

[2]  Teresa M. Amabile,et al.  ? + ? = creativity. , 2018, Public health nursing.

[3]  Robert G. Bednarik,et al.  Creativity in Human Evolution and Prehistory. , 2000 .

[4]  Steven Mithen,et al.  Creativity in Human Evolution and Prehistory , 2005 .

[5]  Liane Gabora,et al.  Contextual Focus: A Cognitive Explanation for the Cultural Revolution of the Middle/Upper Paleolithic , 2013, 1309.2609.

[6]  L. Gabora Revenge of the “Neurds”: Characterizing Creative Thought in Terms of the Structure and Dynamics of Memory , 2010, 1308.5037.

[7]  Liane Gabora,et al.  Honing Theory: A Complex Systems Framework for Creativity. , 2016, Nonlinear dynamics, psychology, and life sciences.

[8]  Steve R. DiPaola,et al.  Incorporating characteristics of human creativity into an evolutionary art algorithm , 2007, GECCO '07.

[9]  Diederik Aerts,et al.  Concepts and Their Dynamics: A Quantum-Theoretic Modeling of Human Thought , 2012, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[10]  Liane Gabora,et al.  A Study and Preliminary Model of Cross-Domain Influences on Creativity , 2015, CogSci.

[11]  Liane Gabora,et al.  A Music-generating System Based on Network Theory , 2016, ICCC.

[12]  Kirsty Kitto,et al.  Concept Combination and the Origins of Complex Cognition , 2013, ArXiv.

[13]  M. Jung-Beeman Bilateral brain processes for comprehending natural language , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[14]  Liane Gabora,et al.  An evolutionary framework for cultural change: selectionism versus communal exchange. , 2012, Physics of life reviews.

[15]  Liane Gabora,et al.  A Computational Model of Two Cognitive Transitions Underlying Cultural Evolution , 2013, CogSci.

[16]  John Kounios,et al.  The Aha! Moment , 2009, Annual review of psychology.

[17]  Diederik Aerts,et al.  A model of the emergence and evolution of integrated worldviews , 2009, 1001.1399.

[18]  Jennifer Culbertson,et al.  Proceedings of the 35th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society , 2013 .

[19]  Jonathan Evans In two minds: dual-process accounts of reasoning , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[20]  L. Gabora,et al.  Cognitive Mechanisms Underlying the Origin and Evolution of Culture , 2001 .

[21]  Steve DiPaola,et al.  Deep Convolutional Networks as Models of Generalization and Blending within Visual Creativity , 2016, ICCC.

[22]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Generalization Through the Recurrent Interaction of Episodic Memories , 2012, Psychological review.

[23]  Mark A. Runco,et al.  The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity: Divergent Thinking, Creativity, and Ideation , 2010 .

[24]  Mark A. Runco,et al.  The Neuroscience of Divergent Thinking , 2014 .

[25]  Charles Dobson,et al.  Evaluative and generative modes of thought during the creative process , 2012, NeuroImage.

[26]  Liane Gabora,et al.  A Tentative Role for FOXP2 in the Evolution of Dual Processing Modes and Generative Abilities , 2014, CogSci.

[27]  Liane Gabora,et al.  Physical Light as a Metaphor for Inner Light , 2014, ArXiv.

[28]  J. Kounios,et al.  The Aha! Moment , 2009 .

[29]  Jordan B. Peterson,et al.  Psychological entropy: a framework for understanding uncertainty-related anxiety. , 2012, Psychological review.

[30]  Jordan B. Peterson,et al.  PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES Decreased Latent Inhibition Is Associated With Increased Creative Achievement in High-Functioning Individuals , 2003 .

[31]  Andrew J. Pringle,et al.  Shifting between modes of thought: a mechanism underlying creative performance? , 2011, C&C '11.

[32]  Derek C. Penn,et al.  Darwin's mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds , 2008, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[33]  Liane Gabora,et al.  How Insight Emerges in a Distributed, Content-addressable Memory , 2011, ArXiv.

[34]  Paul T. Sowden,et al.  Shifting between modes of thought: a domain-general creative thinking skill? , 2016 .

[35]  E. Capaldi,et al.  The organization of behavior. , 1992, Journal of applied behavior analysis.