Separating Storage from Retrieval Dysfunction of Temporal Memory in Parkinson's Disease

Dysfunction of the basal ganglia and the brain nuclei interconnected with them leads to disturbances of movement and cognition exemplified in Parkinson's disease (PD) and Huntington's disease, including disordered timing of movements and impaired time estimation. Previous research has shown that whereas striatal damage in animals can result in the loss of temporal control over behavior, dopaminergic deregulation in the human striatum associated with PD distorts the memory for time. Here we show a dissociation between deficits in storage (writing to) and retrieval (reading from) temporal memory processes. Both are dysfunctional in PD and sensitive to treatment with dopaminergic agents, but produce dissimilar distortions. When time intervals are stored in memory while the subjects are dopamine depleted, the process is slowed, leading to overestimation of two different time intervals. Conversely, when retrieval occurs in a dopamine-depleted state, interference or coupling occurs between two remembered time intervals, producing overestimation of the shorter and underestimation of the longer one. Whether those two separable patterns of dysfunction in storing and retrieving temporal memories rely on distinct neural networks within the basal ganglia and/or their cortical targets remains to be answered by future research.

[1]  S. Keele,et al.  Does the Cerebellum Provide a Common Computation for Diverse Tasks? A Timing Hypothesis a , 1990, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[2]  R M Church,et al.  Scalar Timing in Memory , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[3]  K. Flowers,et al.  Motor set in Parkinson's disease. , 1990, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[4]  John Gibbon,et al.  Ubiquity of scalar timing with Poisson clock , 1992 .

[5]  C. Marsden,et al.  Performance of simultaneous movements in patients with Parkinson's disease. , 1986, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[6]  M. Jahanshahi,et al.  Time estimation and reproduction is abnormal in Parkinson's disease. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[7]  F. Vidal,et al.  Selective effects of low-dose dopamine D1 and D2 receptor antagonists on rat information processing , 2003, Behavioural pharmacology.

[8]  J. Gibbon,et al.  Scalar expectancy theory and peak-interval timing in humans. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[9]  K. Flowers Visual "closed-loop" and "open-loop" characteristics of voluntary movement in patients with Parkinsonism and intention tremor. , 1976, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[10]  R. Church,et al.  Hippocampus, time, and memory. , 1984, Behavioral neuroscience.

[11]  R. Church,et al.  Attention and the frontal cortex as examined by simultaneous temporal processing , 1988, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  S. Roberts,et al.  Isolation of an internal clock. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[13]  J Gibbon,et al.  Cerebellar dysfunctions of temporal processing in the seconds range in humans , 1998, Neuroreport.

[14]  J. Volkmann,et al.  Impairment of temporal organization of speech in basal ganglia diseases , 1992, Brain and Language.

[15]  R. Church,et al.  Methamphetamine and time estimation. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[16]  J A Obeso,et al.  Temporal discrimination is abnormal in Parkinson's disease. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  C. Marsden,et al.  The functions of the basal ganglia and the paradox of stereotaxic surgery in Parkinson's disease. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[18]  S. Keele,et al.  Motor Disorder and the Timing of Repetitive Movements a , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[19]  C. Frith,et al.  Impairments in the learning and performance of a new manual skill in patients with Parkinson's disease. , 1986, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[20]  W. Meck Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[21]  Gordon D A Brown,et al.  A timing-specific memory distortion effect in young children. , 2004, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[22]  M. Nicolelis,et al.  Interval timing and the encoding of signal duration by ensembles of cortical and striatal neurons. , 2003, Behavioral neuroscience.

[23]  J A Obeso,et al.  Basal ganglia pathophysiology. A critical review. , 1997, Advances in neurology.

[24]  S. Keele,et al.  Timing Functions of The Cerebellum , 1989, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  B. Dubois,et al.  Dopamine and cognitive function , 2003, Current opinion in neurology.

[26]  H. Narabayashi,et al.  Disturbances of Rhythm Formation in Patients with Parkinson's Disease: Part I. Characteristics of Tapping Response to the Periodic Signals , 1978, Perceptual and motor skills.

[27]  D. Dick,et al.  Reaction times and attention in Parkinson's disease. , 1987, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[28]  J. Horvitz,et al.  Effects of dopamine antagonists on the timing of two intervals , 2003, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[29]  D. O'Boyle Chapter 11 On the human neuropsychology of timing of simple, repetitive movements , 1997 .

[30]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Toward a neurobiology of temporal cognition: advances and challenges , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[31]  F. W. Cody,et al.  The accuracy and precision of timing of self-paced, repetitive movements in subjects with Parkinson's disease. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[32]  J. Saint-Cyr,et al.  Frontal lobe dysfunction in Parkinson's disease. The cortical focus of neostriatal outflow. , 1986, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[33]  M. Hallett,et al.  A physiological mechanism of bradykinesia. , 1980, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[34]  Y. Agid,et al.  Impaired simultaneous cognitive task performance in Parkinson's disease , 1994, Neurology.

[35]  S. Fairhurst,et al.  Scalar timing in animals and humans , 2002 .

[36]  J A Obeso,et al.  Performance of repetitive wrist movements in Parkinson's disease. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[37]  E V Evarts,et al.  Reaction time in Parkinson's disease. , 1981, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[38]  J. Gibbon Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. , 1977 .

[39]  S. Dollfus,et al.  Verbal learning and memory in schizophrenic and Parkinson's disease patients , 2003, Psychiatry Research.

[40]  M. Morris,et al.  Review article: the functions of the basal ganglia and the paradox of stereotaxic surgery in Parkinson's disease. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[41]  W. N. Schoenfeld The Theory of reinforcement schedules , 1970 .

[42]  J. Gibbon,et al.  Coupled Temporal Memories in Parkinson's Disease: A Dopamine-Related Dysfunction , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[43]  G E Stelmach,et al.  Practice effects on the preprogramming of discrete movements in Parkinson's disease. , 1990, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[44]  Warren H. Meck,et al.  Affinity for the dopamine D2 receptor predicts neuroleptic potency in decreasing the speed of an internal clock , 1986, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[45]  Warren H. Meck,et al.  Chapter 10 How time flies: Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing , 1997 .