Neural Extrapolation of Motion for a Ball Rolling Down an Inclined Plane

It is known that humans tend to misjudge the kinematics of a target rolling down an inclined plane. Because visuomotor responses are often more accurate and less prone to perceptual illusions than cognitive judgments, we asked the question of how rolling motion is extrapolated for manual interception or drawing tasks. In three experiments a ball rolled down an incline with kinematics that differed as a function of the starting position (4 different positions) and slope (30°, 45° or 60°). In Experiment 1, participants had to punch the ball as it fell off the incline. In Experiment 2, the ball rolled down the incline but was stopped at the end; participants were asked to imagine that the ball kept moving and to punch it. In Experiment 3, the ball rolled down the incline and was stopped at the end; participants were asked to draw with the hand in air the trajectory that would be described by the ball if it kept moving. We found that performance was most accurate when motion of the ball was visible until interception and haptic feedback of hand-ball contact was available (Experiment 1). However, even when participants punched an imaginary moving ball (Experiment 2) or drew in air the imaginary trajectory (Experiment 3), they were able to extrapolate to some extent global aspects of the target motion, including its path, speed and arrival time. We argue that the path and kinematics of a ball rolling down an incline can be extrapolated surprisingly well by the brain using both visual information and internal models of target motion.

[1]  Hiromu Katsumata,et al.  Prospective versus predictive control in timing of hitting a falling ball , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  Ian M Franks,et al.  The utilization of visual feedback in the control of movement direction: evidence from a video aiming task. , 2003, Motor control.

[3]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Internal models of target motion: expected dynamics overrides measured kinematics in timing manual interceptions. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  Daeyeol Lee,et al.  Manual interception of moving targets I. Performance and movement initiation , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  P. McLeod Visual Reaction Time and High-Speed Ball Games , 1987, Perception.

[6]  M. Bertaminia,et al.  The representation of naïve knowledge about physics , 2005 .

[7]  T. Hubbard Representational momentum and related displacements in spatial memory: A review of the findings , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[8]  D. Proffitt,et al.  Perceived Slant: A Dissociation between Perception and Action , 2007, Perception.

[9]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Anticipatory and reflex coactivation of antagonist muscles in catching , 1987, Brain Research.

[10]  D. Proffitt,et al.  Understanding natural dynamics. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  Francesco Lacquaniti,et al.  Catching a Ball at the Right Time and Place: Individual Factors Matter , 2012, PloS one.

[12]  C. Craig,et al.  Bending It Like Beckham: How to Visually Fool the Goalkeeper , 2010, PloS one.

[13]  S. Bennett,et al.  Is Acceleration Used for Ocular Pursuit and Spatial Estimation during Prediction Motion? , 2013, PloS one.

[14]  Vincenzo Maffei,et al.  Extrapolation of vertical target motion through a brief visual occlusion , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  Mark F. Bradshaw,et al.  Reaching for virtual objects: binocular disparity and the control of prehension , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  J. Tresilian,et al.  a moving target: effects of temporal precision constraints and movement amplitude , 2022 .

[17]  J. Tresilian Visually timed action: time-out for ‘tau’? , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  L Proteau,et al.  Determining movement onsets from temporal series. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[19]  P. Morasso Spatial control of arm movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  P Bressan,et al.  Wheels: A New Illusion in the Perception of Rolling Objects , 1990, Perception.

[21]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Adaptation to suppression of visual information during catching , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[22]  Stephen J Heinen,et al.  Perceptual and oculomotor evidence of limitations on processing accelerating motion. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[23]  Simon J. Bennett,et al.  Advance knowledge effects on kinematics of one-handed catching , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  KNOWLEDGE ON ACCELERATED MOTION AS MEASURED BY IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT TASKS IN 5 TO 16 YEAR OLDS , 2011 .

[25]  Eliane C Magdalon,et al.  Comparison of grasping movements made by healthy subjects in a 3-dimensional immersive virtual versus physical environment. , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[26]  M. Goodale,et al.  Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand , 1995, Current Biology.

[27]  Jeanine K. Stefanucci,et al.  Seeing beyond the Target: Environmental Context Affects Distance Perception , 2007, Perception.

[28]  Francesco Lacquaniti,et al.  Internal model of gravity for hand interception: parametric adaptation to zero-gravity visual targets on Earth. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  Anne-Marie Brouwer,et al.  Hitting moving targets , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  Dirk Kerzel,et al.  Like a rolling stone: naturalistic visual kinematics facilitate tracking eye movements. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[31]  Welber Marinovic,et al.  Neural prediction of complex accelerations for object interception. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  Dale Purves,et al.  Natural-scene geometry predicts the perception of angles and line orientation. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[33]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Target Interception: Hand–Eye Coordination and Strategies , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  N. A. Borghese,et al.  Time-varying mechanical behavior of multijointed arm in man. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  Daeyeol Lee,et al.  Manual interception of moving targets II. On-line control of overlapping submovements , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  Rich Gossweiler,et al.  Perceiving geographical slant , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[37]  E. Vaadia,et al.  Differences in Context and Feedback Result in Different Trajectories and Adaptation Strategies in Reaching , 2009, PloS one.

[38]  Heiko Hecht,et al.  The Effect of Body Posture on Long-Range Time-to-Contact Estimation , 2011, Perception.

[39]  H. Hecht Judging Rolling Wheels: Dynamic and Kinematic Aspects of Rotation – Translation Coupling , 1993, Perception.

[40]  H. Hecht,et al.  Can representational trajectory reveal the nature of an internal model of gravity? , 2014, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[41]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Representation of Visual Gravitational Motion in the Human Vestibular Cortex , 2005, Science.

[42]  Francesco Lacquaniti,et al.  Catching What We Can't See: Manual Interception of Occluded Fly-Ball Trajectories , 2012, PloS one.

[43]  Joan López-Moliner,et al.  Synergies between optical and physical variables in intercepting parabolic targets , 2013, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[44]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Curvilinear motion in the absence of external forces: naive beliefs about the motion of objects. , 1980, Science.

[45]  C. Michaels,et al.  Information and action in punching a falling ball , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[46]  William J. Wilson,et al.  Multivariate Statistical Methods , 2005, Technometrics.

[47]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Predicting curvilinear target motion through an occlusion , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[48]  R. Shepard Ecological constraints on internal representation: resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining, thinking, and dreaming. , 1984, Psychological review.

[49]  Grant Malcolm Multidisciplinary Approaches to Visual Representations and Interpretations , 2004 .

[50]  Joost C. Dessing,et al.  Adaptations of lateral hand movements to early and late visual occlusion in catching , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[51]  H. Hecht,et al.  Influence of animation on dynamical judgments. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[52]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Timing the moment of impact in fast human movements. , 2012, Acta psychologica.

[53]  Peter M. Vishton,et al.  Timing of anticipatory muscle tensing control: responses before and after expected impact , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[54]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Viewer-centered frame of reference for pointing to memorized targets in three-dimensional space. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[55]  P. Beek,et al.  A Comparison of Real Catching With Catching Using Stereoscopic Visual Displays , 2004 .

[56]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Does the brain model Newton's laws? , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[57]  D. Rohrer Misconceptions about incline speed for nonlinear slopes. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[58]  David N. Lee,et al.  Visual Timing in Hitting An Accelerating Ball , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[59]  Maggie Shiffrar,et al.  Rolling Perception without Rolling Motion , 2008, Perception.

[60]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Internal models and prediction of visual gravitational motion , 2008, Vision Research.

[61]  Heiko Hecht,et al.  The representational dynamics of remembered projectile locations. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[62]  Welber Marinovic,et al.  The utilisation of visual information in the control of rapid interceptive actions. , 2009, Experimental psychology.

[63]  G E Stelmach,et al.  Stability of reach-to-grasp movement patterns in Parkinson's disease. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[64]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Fast adaptation of the internal model of gravity for manual interceptions: evidence for event-dependent learning. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[65]  E. Brenner,et al.  Hitting moving targets Continuous control of the acceleration of the hand on the basis of the target’s velocity , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[66]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Cognitive, perceptual and action-oriented representations of falling objects , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[67]  P. Werkhoven,et al.  Visual processing of optic acceleration , 1992, Vision Research.

[68]  D. Proffitt,et al.  Understanding wheel dynamics , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.

[69]  T. Hubbard,et al.  Environmental invariants in the representation of motion: Implied dynamics and representational momentum, gravity, friction, and centripetal force , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.