Neural Extrapolation of Motion for a Ball Rolling Down an Inclined Plane
暂无分享,去创建一个
Francesco Lacquaniti | Barbara La Scaleia | Myrka Zago | F. Lacquaniti | M. Zago | Barbara La Scaleia
[1] Hiromu Katsumata,et al. Prospective versus predictive control in timing of hitting a falling ball , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.
[2] Ian M Franks,et al. The utilization of visual feedback in the control of movement direction: evidence from a video aiming task. , 2003, Motor control.
[3] F. Lacquaniti,et al. Internal models of target motion: expected dynamics overrides measured kinematics in timing manual interceptions. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.
[4] Daeyeol Lee,et al. Manual interception of moving targets I. Performance and movement initiation , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.
[5] P. McLeod. Visual Reaction Time and High-Speed Ball Games , 1987, Perception.
[6] M. Bertaminia,et al. The representation of naïve knowledge about physics , 2005 .
[7] T. Hubbard. Representational momentum and related displacements in spatial memory: A review of the findings , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[8] D. Proffitt,et al. Perceived Slant: A Dissociation between Perception and Action , 2007, Perception.
[9] F. Lacquaniti,et al. Anticipatory and reflex coactivation of antagonist muscles in catching , 1987, Brain Research.
[10] D. Proffitt,et al. Understanding natural dynamics. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[11] Francesco Lacquaniti,et al. Catching a Ball at the Right Time and Place: Individual Factors Matter , 2012, PloS one.
[12] C. Craig,et al. Bending It Like Beckham: How to Visually Fool the Goalkeeper , 2010, PloS one.
[13] S. Bennett,et al. Is Acceleration Used for Ocular Pursuit and Spatial Estimation during Prediction Motion? , 2013, PloS one.
[14] Vincenzo Maffei,et al. Extrapolation of vertical target motion through a brief visual occlusion , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[15] Mark F. Bradshaw,et al. Reaching for virtual objects: binocular disparity and the control of prehension , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[16] J. Tresilian,et al. a moving target: effects of temporal precision constraints and movement amplitude , 2022 .
[17] J. Tresilian. Visually timed action: time-out for ‘tau’? , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[18] L Proteau,et al. Determining movement onsets from temporal series. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.
[19] P. Morasso. Spatial control of arm movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[20] P Bressan,et al. Wheels: A New Illusion in the Perception of Rolling Objects , 1990, Perception.
[21] F. Lacquaniti,et al. Adaptation to suppression of visual information during catching , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[22] Stephen J Heinen,et al. Perceptual and oculomotor evidence of limitations on processing accelerating motion. , 2003, Journal of vision.
[23] Simon J. Bennett,et al. Advance knowledge effects on kinematics of one-handed catching , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[24] Lieven Verschaffel,et al. KNOWLEDGE ON ACCELERATED MOTION AS MEASURED BY IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT TASKS IN 5 TO 16 YEAR OLDS , 2011 .
[25] Eliane C Magdalon,et al. Comparison of grasping movements made by healthy subjects in a 3-dimensional immersive virtual versus physical environment. , 2011, Acta psychologica.
[26] M. Goodale,et al. Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand , 1995, Current Biology.
[27] Jeanine K. Stefanucci,et al. Seeing beyond the Target: Environmental Context Affects Distance Perception , 2007, Perception.
[28] Francesco Lacquaniti,et al. Internal model of gravity for hand interception: parametric adaptation to zero-gravity visual targets on Earth. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.
[29] Anne-Marie Brouwer,et al. Hitting moving targets , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.
[30] Dirk Kerzel,et al. Like a rolling stone: naturalistic visual kinematics facilitate tracking eye movements. , 2013, Journal of vision.
[31] Welber Marinovic,et al. Neural prediction of complex accelerations for object interception. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.
[32] Dale Purves,et al. Natural-scene geometry predicts the perception of angles and line orientation. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[33] J. F. Soechting,et al. Target Interception: Hand–Eye Coordination and Strategies , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[34] N. A. Borghese,et al. Time-varying mechanical behavior of multijointed arm in man. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.
[35] Daeyeol Lee,et al. Manual interception of moving targets II. On-line control of overlapping submovements , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.
[36] Rich Gossweiler,et al. Perceiving geographical slant , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[37] E. Vaadia,et al. Differences in Context and Feedback Result in Different Trajectories and Adaptation Strategies in Reaching , 2009, PloS one.
[38] Heiko Hecht,et al. The Effect of Body Posture on Long-Range Time-to-Contact Estimation , 2011, Perception.
[39] H. Hecht. Judging Rolling Wheels: Dynamic and Kinematic Aspects of Rotation – Translation Coupling , 1993, Perception.
[40] H. Hecht,et al. Can representational trajectory reveal the nature of an internal model of gravity? , 2014, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[41] F. Lacquaniti,et al. Representation of Visual Gravitational Motion in the Human Vestibular Cortex , 2005, Science.
[42] Francesco Lacquaniti,et al. Catching What We Can't See: Manual Interception of Occluded Fly-Ball Trajectories , 2012, PloS one.
[43] Joan López-Moliner,et al. Synergies between optical and physical variables in intercepting parabolic targets , 2013, Front. Behav. Neurosci..
[44] A. Caramazza,et al. Curvilinear motion in the absence of external forces: naive beliefs about the motion of objects. , 1980, Science.
[45] C. Michaels,et al. Information and action in punching a falling ball , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[46] William J. Wilson,et al. Multivariate Statistical Methods , 2005, Technometrics.
[47] J. F. Soechting,et al. Predicting curvilinear target motion through an occlusion , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.
[48] R. Shepard. Ecological constraints on internal representation: resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining, thinking, and dreaming. , 1984, Psychological review.
[49] Grant Malcolm. Multidisciplinary Approaches to Visual Representations and Interpretations , 2004 .
[50] Joost C. Dessing,et al. Adaptations of lateral hand movements to early and late visual occlusion in catching , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.
[51] H. Hecht,et al. Influence of animation on dynamical judgments. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[52] Eli Brenner,et al. Timing the moment of impact in fast human movements. , 2012, Acta psychologica.
[53] Peter M. Vishton,et al. Timing of anticipatory muscle tensing control: responses before and after expected impact , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[54] F. Lacquaniti,et al. Viewer-centered frame of reference for pointing to memorized targets in three-dimensional space. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.
[55] P. Beek,et al. A Comparison of Real Catching With Catching Using Stereoscopic Visual Displays , 2004 .
[56] F. Lacquaniti,et al. Does the brain model Newton's laws? , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[57] D. Rohrer. Misconceptions about incline speed for nonlinear slopes. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[58] David N. Lee,et al. Visual Timing in Hitting An Accelerating Ball , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[59] Maggie Shiffrar,et al. Rolling Perception without Rolling Motion , 2008, Perception.
[60] F. Lacquaniti,et al. Internal models and prediction of visual gravitational motion , 2008, Vision Research.
[61] Heiko Hecht,et al. The representational dynamics of remembered projectile locations. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[62] Welber Marinovic,et al. The utilisation of visual information in the control of rapid interceptive actions. , 2009, Experimental psychology.
[63] G E Stelmach,et al. Stability of reach-to-grasp movement patterns in Parkinson's disease. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[64] F. Lacquaniti,et al. Fast adaptation of the internal model of gravity for manual interceptions: evidence for event-dependent learning. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.
[65] E. Brenner,et al. Hitting moving targets Continuous control of the acceleration of the hand on the basis of the target’s velocity , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.
[66] F. Lacquaniti,et al. Cognitive, perceptual and action-oriented representations of falling objects , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[67] P. Werkhoven,et al. Visual processing of optic acceleration , 1992, Vision Research.
[68] D. Proffitt,et al. Understanding wheel dynamics , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.
[69] T. Hubbard,et al. Environmental invariants in the representation of motion: Implied dynamics and representational momentum, gravity, friction, and centripetal force , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.