The role of item-specific information for the serial position curve in free recall.

The serial position curve in free recall of a list of action phrases differs depending on whether the phrases were memorized by listening/reading (verbal task; VT) or by additionally enacting the denoted actions (subject-performed task; SPT). In VTs there is a clear primacy effect and a short recency effect. In SPTs there is no primacy effect but an extended recency effect. H. D. Zimmer, T. Helstrup, and J. Engelkamp (2000) assumed that SPTs provide excellent item-specific information, which leads to an automatic pop-out of the items presented last. In the present study, the authors assumed that good item-specific encoding generally enhances the recency effect and that it hinders rehearsal processes and thereby reduces the primacy effect. This assumption was confirmed. An item-specific orienting task leads to parallel serial position curves in VTs and SPTs with no primacy effect but a clear recency effect. Moreover, the same serial position effects were shown with nouns as learning material. An item-specific orienting task changes the classical U-shaped serial position curve with verbal material and leads to the disappearance of the primacy and the enhancement of the recency effect.

[1]  M. Glanzer,et al.  Two storage mechanisms in free recall , 1966 .

[2]  R. Atkinson,et al.  Effects of overt rehearsal procedures on free recall , 1970 .

[3]  D. Rundus Analysis of rehearsal processes in free recall. , 1971 .

[4]  R. Bjork,et al.  Recency-sensitive retrieval processes in long-term free recall☆ , 1974 .

[5]  R. G. Crowder Principles of learning and memory , 1977 .

[6]  M. Bradley,et al.  A two-process account of long-term serial position effects , 1980 .

[7]  B. Fischhoff,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory , 1980 .

[8]  Suzanne Donnenwerth-Nolan,et al.  Does motoric imagery facilitate memory for sentences? A selective interference test , 1981 .

[9]  G. Einstein,et al.  Relational and item-specific information in memory , 1981 .

[10]  R. L. Cohen,et al.  The effect of encoding variables on the free recall of words and action events , 1983, Memory & cognition.

[11]  Lars-Göran Nilsson,et al.  Aging effects in free recall: An exception to the rule. , 1984 .

[12]  L. Bäckman,et al.  Prerequisites for lack of age differences in memory performance. , 1985, Experimental aging research.

[13]  Mark A. McDaniel,et al.  Encoding difficulty and memory: toward a unifying theory , 1986 .

[14]  Johannes Engelkamp,et al.  Nouns and verbs in paired-associate learning: Instructional effects , 1986 .

[15]  John C. Yuille,et al.  The Role of Imagery in Memory: On Shared and Distinctive Information , 1987 .

[16]  T. Helstrup,et al.  One, two, or three memories? A problem-solving approach to memory for performed acts ☆ , 1987 .

[17]  P. E. Morris,et al.  Practical aspects of memory : current research and issues , 1988 .

[18]  J. Kihlstrom,et al.  Effects of item-specific and relational information on hypermnesic recall. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[19]  M. Knopf Having shaved a kiwi fruit: Memory of unfamiliar subject-performed actions , 1991 .

[20]  I. Neath Distinctiveness and serial position effects in recognition , 1993, Memory & cognition.

[21]  D. J. Burns,et al.  Item gains and losses during hypermnesic recall: Implications for the item-specific-relational information distinction. , 1993 .

[22]  J. Bortz Statistik: Fur Sozialwissenschaftler , 1993 .

[23]  Roderick Hunt,et al.  The Enigma of Organization and Distinctiveness , 1993 .

[24]  A. Thapar,et al.  Effects of level of processing on implicit and explicit tasks. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[25]  Reza Kormi-Nouri,et al.  The nature of memory for action events: An episodic integration view , 1995 .

[26]  J Engelkamp,et al.  Visual imagery and enactment of actions in memory. , 1995, British journal of psychology.

[27]  M. J. Sharps,et al.  Visual and auditory information as determinants of primacy effects. , 1996, The Journal of general psychology.

[28]  U. Olofsson Win some, lose some: Hypermnesia for actions reflects increased item-specific processing , 1997, Memory & cognition.

[29]  Hubert D. Zimmer,et al.  Sensory factors in memory for subject-performed tasks , 1997 .

[30]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Dynamic changes in hypermnesia across early and late tests: a relational/item-specific account. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[31]  J. Engelkamp Memory for actions , 1998 .

[32]  K. Schoff,et al.  Slow and steady often ties the race: Effects of item-specific and relational processing on cumulative recall. , 1998 .

[33]  J Engelkamp,et al.  Levels-of-processing effects in subject-performed tasks , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[34]  G. Ward,et al.  A recency-based account of the primacy effect in free recall. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[35]  J Engelkamp,et al.  Pop-out into memory: a retrieval mechanism that is enhanced with the recall of subject-performed tasks. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[36]  Lars-Göran Nilsson,et al.  Remembering actions and words , 2000 .

[37]  Lars-Göran Nilsson,et al.  Reactivation of Motor Brain Areas during Explicit Memory for Actions , 2001, NeuroImage.

[38]  J. Rouder,et al.  Modelling serial position curves with temporal distinctiveness , 2001, Memory.

[39]  Hubert D. Zimmer,et al.  Memory for Action: A Distinct Form of Episodic Memory? , 2001 .

[40]  Johannes Engelkamp,et al.  Gains and Losses in Action Memory , 2003, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[41]  F. Craik,et al.  The Oxford handbook of memory , 2006 .