Surface-Illuminant Ambiguity and Color Constancy: Effects of Scene Complexity and Depth Cues

Two experiments were conducted to study how scene complexity and cues to depth affect human color constancy. Specifically, two levels of scene complexity were compared. The low-complexity scene contained two walls with the same surface reflectance and a test patch which provided no information about the illuminant. In addition to the surfaces visible in the low-complexity scene, the high-complexity scene contained two rectangular solid objects and 24 paper samples with diverse surface reflectances. Observers viewed illuminated objects in an experimental chamber and adjusted the test patch until it appeared achromatic. Achromatic settings made under two different illuminants were used to compute an index that quantified the degree of constancy. Two experiments were conducted: one in which observers viewed the stimuli directly, and one in which they viewed the scenes through an optical system that reduced cues to depth. In each experiment, constancy was assessed for two conditions. In the valid-cue condition, many cues provided valid information about the illuminant change. In the invalid-cue condition, some image cues provided invalid information. Four broad conclusions are drawn from the data: (a) constancy is generally better in the valid-cue condition than in the invalid-cue condition; (b) for the stimulus configuration used, increasing image complexity has little effect in the valid-cue condition but leads to increased constancy in the invalid-cue condition; (c) for the stimulus configuration used, reducing cues to depth has little effect for either constancy condition; and (d) there is moderate individual variation in the degree of constancy exhibited, particularly in the degree to which the complexity manipulation affects performance.

[1]  Jon M. Speigle,et al.  Predicting color from gray: the relationship between achromatic adjustment and asymmetric matching. , 1999, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[2]  D H Brainard,et al.  Color constancy in the nearly natural image. I. Asymmetric matches. , 1997, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[3]  L. Arend,et al.  Simultaneous color constancy. , 1986, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[4]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[5]  Robert W. Burnham,et al.  Prediction of Color Appearance with Different Adaptation Illuminations , 1957 .

[6]  L. Maloney,et al.  Color constancy: a method for recovering surface spectral reflectance. , 1986, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[7]  Journal of the Optical Society of America , 1950, Nature.

[8]  Karl-Heinz Bäuml,et al.  PII: S0042-6989(98)00192-8 , 1998 .

[9]  A. Gelb Die „Farbenkonstanz“ der Sehdinge , 1929 .

[10]  A. Valberg,et al.  “Colour constancy” in Mondrian patterns: A partial cancellation of physical chromaticity shifts by simultaneous contrast , 1990, Vision Research.

[11]  D. Brainard,et al.  Color constancy in the nearly natural image. 2. Achromatic loci. , 1998, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[12]  Michael D'Zmura,et al.  Geometric Representations of Perceptual Phenomena : Papers in Honor of Tarow indow on His 70th Birthday , 1995 .

[13]  M. Gazzaniga,et al.  The new cognitive neurosciences , 2000 .

[14]  C. M. D. de Weert,et al.  Transparent layer constancy. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  D. Brainard,et al.  Mechanisms of color constancy under nearly natural viewing. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  Vincent Walsh,et al.  Perceptual Constancy: why things look as they do , 1998 .

[17]  David H. Brainard,et al.  Control of chromatic adaptation: signals from separate cone classes interact , 2000, Vision Research.

[18]  A. Hurlbert Colour vision: Is colour constancy real? , 1999, Current Biology.

[19]  I Rock,et al.  Is a Perceived Shape Based on its Retinal Image? , 1993, Perception.

[20]  D H Brainard,et al.  Bayesian color constancy. , 1997, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[21]  D H Brainard,et al.  Analysis of the retinex theory of color vision. , 1986, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[22]  E. Chichilnisky,et al.  Seeing gray through the ON and OFF pathways , 1996, Visual Neuroscience.

[23]  A. Gilchrist,et al.  When does perceived lightness depend on perceived spatial arrangement? , 1980, Perception & psychophysics.

[24]  A. Bethe,et al.  Handbuch der Normalen und Pathologischen Physiologie , 1925 .

[25]  M. Gazzaniga The new cognitive neurosciences, 2nd ed. , 2000 .

[26]  A. Gilchrist,et al.  An anchoring theory of lightness perception. , 1999 .

[27]  M. D'Zmura,et al.  Color Transparency , 1997, Perception.

[28]  G. Buchsbaum A spatial processor model for object colour perception , 1980 .

[29]  A. Hurlbert,et al.  Perception of three-dimensional shape influences colour perception through mutual illumination , 1999, Nature.

[30]  L. Maloney Physics-based approaches to modeling surface color perception , 1999 .

[31]  A. Gilchrist Perceived lightness depends on perceived spatial arrangement. , 1977, Science.

[32]  B. Wandell,et al.  Asymmetric color matching: how color appearance depends on the illuminant. , 1992, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[33]  Karl R. Gegenfurtner,et al.  Color Vision: From Genes to Perception , 1999 .

[34]  F Metelli,et al.  The perception of transparency. , 1974, Scientific American.

[35]  D. Knill,et al.  Apparent surface curvature affects lightness perception , 1991, Nature.

[36]  H. Helson,et al.  The effect of chromatic adaptation on achromaticity. , 1948, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[37]  M. Lucassen,et al.  Quantifying color constancy: Evidence for nonlinear processing of cone-specific contrast , 1993, Vision Research.

[38]  R. Henneman,et al.  A photometric study of the perception of object color , 1935 .

[39]  P. W. Trezona Luminance level conversions to assist lighting engineers to use fundamental visual data , 1983 .

[40]  L. Pollini,et al.  AM A A , 2000 .

[41]  L. Arend,et al.  Perceived lightness, but not brightness, of achromatic surfaces depends on perceived depth information , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[42]  A. Gilchrist,et al.  An anchoring theory of lightness perception. , 1999, Psychological review.

[43]  J. Werner,et al.  Effect of chromatic adaptation on the achromatic locus: The role of contrast, luminance and background color , 1982, Vision Research.

[44]  M. D'Zmura,et al.  Color constancy. I. Basic theory of two-stage linear recovery of spectral descriptions for lights and surfaces. , 1993, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[45]  M. Gazzaniga The new cognitive neurosciences, 2nd edition , 2002 .

[46]  E. Adelson Lightness Perception and Lightness Illusions , 1999 .

[47]  Tiziano Agostini,et al.  Lightness contrast in CRT and paper-and-illuminant displays , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[48]  A. Gilchrist,et al.  Perception of Lightness and Illumination in a World of One Reflectance , 1984, Perception.

[49]  Joongnam Yang Illuminant estimation in surface color perception , 1999 .

[50]  S. McKee,et al.  Quantitative studies in retinex theory a comparison between theoretical predictions and observer responses to the “color mondrian” experiments , 1976, Vision Research.

[51]  K. Bäuml Color appearance: effects of illuminant changes under different surface collections. , 1994, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[52]  Brainard,et al.  Colour constancy: developing empirical tests of computational models , 2003 .