Sleep deprivation alters functioning within the neural network underlying the covert orienting of attention

One function of spatial attention is to enable goal-directed interactions with the environment through the allocation of neural resources to motivationally relevant parts of space. Studies have shown that responses are enhanced when spatial attention is predictively biased towards locations where significant events are expected to occur. Previous studies suggest that the ability to bias attention predictively is related to posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) activation [Small, D.M., et al., 2003. The posterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex mediate the anticipatory allocation of spatial attention. Neuroimage 18, 633-41]. Sleep deprivation (SD) impairs selective attention and reduces PCC activity [Thomas, M., et al., 2000. Neural basis of alertness and cognitive performance impairments during sleepiness. I. Effects of 24 h of sleep deprivation on waking human regional brain activity. J. Sleep Res. 9, 335-352]. Based on these findings, we hypothesized that SD would affect PCC function and alter the ability to predictively allocate spatial attention. Seven healthy, young adults underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) following normal rest and 34-36 h of SD while performing a task in which attention was shifted in response to peripheral targets preceded by spatially informative (valid), misleading (invalid), or uninformative (neutral) cues. When rested, but not when sleep-deprived, subjects responded more quickly to targets that followed valid cues than those after neutral or invalid cues. Brain activity during validly cued trials with a reaction time benefit was compared to activity in trials with no benefit. PCC activation was greater during trials with a reaction time benefit following normal rest. In contrast, following SD, reaction time benefits were associated with activation in the left intraparietal sulcus, a region associated with receptivity to stimuli at unexpected locations. These changes may render sleep-deprived individuals less able to anticipate the locations of upcoming events, and more susceptible to distraction by stimuli at irrelevant locations.

[1]  Daniel J Buysse,et al.  Human regional cerebral glucose metabolism during non-rapid eye movement sleep in relation to waking. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[2]  Gregory G. Brown,et al.  Increased cerebral response during a divided attention task following sleep deprivation , 2001, Journal of sleep research.

[3]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Nonstationary cluster-size inference with random field and permutation methods , 2004, NeuroImage.

[4]  Gregory G. Brown,et al.  Altered brain response to verbal learning following sleep deprivation , 2000, Nature.

[5]  Steven Laureys,et al.  Posterior cingulate, precuneal and retrosplenial cortices: cytology and components of the neural network correlates of consciousness. , 2005, Progress in brain research.

[6]  D. Dawson,et al.  Fatigue, alcohol and performance impairment , 1997, Nature.

[7]  M. Buchsbaum,et al.  The effect of sleep deprivation on cerebral glucose metabolic rate in normal humans assessed with positron emission tomography. , 1991, Sleep.

[8]  L. Mulder,et al.  Visual selective attention during meaningful noise and after sleep deprivation. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[9]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[10]  W. F. Waters,et al.  Decreased attentional responsivity during sleep deprivation: orienting response latency, amplitude, and habituation. , 1997, Sleep.

[11]  Rajesh Kumar,et al.  A method for removal of global effects from fMRI time series , 2004, NeuroImage.

[12]  M. Chee,et al.  Functional Imaging of Working Memory after 24 Hr of Total Sleep Deprivation , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  D. Dinges,et al.  Performing while sleepy: Effects of experimentally-induced sleepiness. , 1991 .

[14]  F. Mauguière,et al.  Attention shifts and anticipatory mechanisms in hyperactive children: an ERP study using the Posner paradigm , 2001, Biological Psychiatry.

[15]  H. L. Williams,et al.  Impaired performance with acute sleep loss. , 1959 .

[16]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Movements of attention in the three spatial dimensions and the meaning of “neutral” cues , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  M. A. Steinmetz,et al.  Neuronal responses in area 7a to multiple stimulus displays: II. responses are suppressed at the cued location. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[18]  Maria L. Thomas,et al.  Neural basis of alertness and cognitive performance impairments during sleepiness. I. Effects of 24 h of sleep deprivation on waking human regional brain activity , 2000, Journal of sleep research.

[19]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerebral activation , 1996, Human brain mapping.

[20]  N. Amir,et al.  Attentional bias to threat in social phobia: facilitated processing of threat or difficulty disengaging attention from threat? , 2003, Behaviour research and therapy.

[21]  M. Strauss,et al.  Functional brain imaging of a complex navigation task following one night of total sleep deprivation: a preliminary study , 2005, Journal of sleep research.

[22]  Joel L. Voss,et al.  Experience-dependent neural integration of taste and smell in the human brain. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Detecting Activations in PET and fMRI: Levels of Inference and Power , 1996, NeuroImage.

[24]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[25]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A Common Network of Functional Areas for Attention and Eye Movements , 1998, Neuron.

[26]  Joel R. Meyer,et al.  A large-scale distributed network for covert spatial attention: further anatomical delineation based on stringent behavioural and cognitive controls. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[27]  T. Monk Sleep, sleepiness, and performance , 1991 .

[28]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  The Inferential Impact of Global Signal Covariates in Functional Neuroimaging Analyses , 1998, NeuroImage.

[29]  A. Nobre,et al.  Heterogeneity of Cingulate Contributions to Spatial Attention , 2001, NeuroImage.

[30]  R Norton,et al.  The effects of acute sleep deprivation on selective attention. , 1970, British journal of psychology.

[31]  F. Smulders,et al.  Varieties of attention in neutral trials: linking RT to ERPs and EEG frequencies. , 2006, Psychophysiology.

[32]  D. Gitelman,et al.  Covert Visual Spatial Orienting and Saccades: Overlapping Neural Systems , 2000, NeuroImage.

[33]  G. Mangun,et al.  Dissociating top-down attentional control from selective perception and action , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  Todd B. Parrish,et al.  The posterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex mediate the anticipatory allocation of spatial attention , 2003, NeuroImage.

[35]  Albrecht W. Inhoff,et al.  Isolating attentional systems: A cognitive-anatomical analysis , 1987, Psychobiology.

[36]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  A M Dale,et al.  Randomized event‐related experimental designs allow for extremely rapid presentation rates using functional MRI , 1998, Neuroreport.

[38]  D. Robinson,et al.  Covert orienting of attention in macaques. II. Contributions of parietal cortex. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  Douglas C. Noll,et al.  Overt Verbal Responding during fMRI Scanning: Empirical Investigations of Problems and Potential Solutions , 1999, NeuroImage.

[40]  M. Mesulam,et al.  Primary progressive aphasia--a language-based dementia. , 2003, The New England journal of medicine.