Issues in dietary intake assessment of children and adolescents

Studies of food habits and dietary intakes face a number of unique respondent and observer considerations at different stages from early childhood to late adolescence. Despite this, intakes have often been reported as if valid, and the interpretation of links between intake and health has been based, often erroneously, on the assumption of validity. However, validation studies of energy intake data have led to the widespread recognition that much of the dietary data on children and adolescents is prone to reporting error, mostly through under-reporting. Reporting error is influenced by body weight status and does not occur systematically across different age groups or different dietary survey techniques. It appears that the available methods for assessing the dietary intakes of children are, at best, able to provide unbiased estimates of energy intake only at the group level, while the food intake data of most adolescents are particularly prone to reporting error at both the group and the individual level. Moreover, evidence for the existence of subject-specific responding in dietary assessments challenges the assumption that repeated measurements of dietary intake will eventually obtain valid data. Only limited progress has been made in understanding the variables associated with misreporting in these age groups, the associated biases in estimating nutrient intakes and the most appropriate way to interpret unrepresentative dietary data. Until these issues are better understood, researchers should exercise considerable caution when evaluating all such data.

[1]  K. Nelson The Psychological and Social Origins of Autobiographical Memory , 1993 .

[2]  J. Stevens,et al.  Portion-size estimation training in second- and third-grade American Indian children. , 1999, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[3]  J. Haraldsdóttir,et al.  Repeated 24-h recalls with young schoolchildren. A feasible alternative to dietary history from parents? , 1995, European Journal of Clinical Nutrition.

[4]  T. Baranowski,et al.  Children's frequency of consumption of foods high in fat and sodium. , 1990, American journal of preventive medicine.

[5]  J. Patrie,et al.  Alterations in growth and body composition during puberty. IV. Energy intake estimated by the youth-adolescent food-frequency questionnaire: validation by the doubly labeled water method. , 2000, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[6]  N Slimani,et al.  Measurement of past diet: review of previous and proposed methods. , 1992, Epidemiologic reviews.

[7]  M. Litaker,et al.  Children's dietary recalls: the salience of entree and liking for foods on accuracy and order of reporting. , 1999, Nutrition.

[8]  S. Kelder,et al.  Longitudinal tracking of adolescent smoking, physical activity, and food choice behaviors. , 1994, American journal of public health.

[9]  R G Whitehead,et al.  Validation of estimates of energy intake by weighed dietary record and diet history in children and adolescents. , 1992, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[10]  Henry A. Feldman,et al.  Perspective in practiceValidation of 24-hour recalls assisted by food records in third-grade children☆ , 1993 .

[11]  T. Baranowski,et al.  The accuracy of children's self-reports of diet: Family Health Project. , 1986, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[12]  A. Eliakim,et al.  Fitness and the effect of exercise training on the dietary intake of healthy adolescents , 1998, International Journal of Obesity.

[13]  W. Coward,et al.  Total energy expenditure and energy intake in the pre-school child: a comparison , 1994, British Journal of Nutrition.

[14]  L. Bandini,et al.  Validity of reported energy intake in preadolescent girls. , 1997, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[15]  L. Emmons,et al.  Accuracy of 24-hr. recalls of young children. , 1973, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[16]  G. Berenson,et al.  Dietary studies of children from a biracial population: intakes of fat and fatty acids in 10- and 13-year olds. , 1984, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[17]  C. Basch,et al.  Validation of mothers' reports of dietary intake by four to seven year-old children. , 1990, American journal of public health.

[18]  C. Champagne,et al.  Assessment of energy intake underreporting by doubly labeled water and observations on reported nutrient intakes in children. , 1998, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[19]  G. Berenson,et al.  Dietary studies of children from a biracial population: intakes of vitamins in 10- and 13-year-olds. , 1985, Journal of the American College of Nutrition.

[20]  M. Singer,et al.  The tracking of nutrient intake in young children: the Framingham Children's Study. , 1995, American journal of public health.

[21]  S. Baxter,et al.  "How do you remember you ate...?": a Delphi technique study to identify retrieval categories from fourth-grade children. , 1997, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[22]  T J Cole,et al.  Biased over- or under-reporting is characteristic of individuals whether over time or by different assessment methods. , 2001, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[23]  D A Schoeller,et al.  Validity of reported energy intake in obese and nonobese adolescents. , 1990, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[24]  L. Rips,et al.  Answering autobiographical questions: the impact of memory and inference on surveys. , 1987, Science.

[25]  J. Reilly,et al.  Reduced Energy Expenditure in Preobese Children Treated for Acute Lymphoblastic Leukemia , 1998, Pediatric Research.

[26]  G Samuelson,et al.  Total energy expenditure and physical activity as assessed by the doubly labeled water method in Swedish adolescents in whom energy intake was underestimated by 7-d diet records. , 1998, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[27]  J. Strain,et al.  An evaluation of the sensitivity and specificity of energy expenditure measured by heart rate and the Goldberg cut-off for energy intake: basal metabolic rate for identifying mis-reporting of energy intake by adults and children: a retrospective analysis , 2003, European Journal of Clinical Nutrition.

[28]  Jane Wardle,et al.  Restraint, body image and food attitudes in children from 12 to 18 years , 1986, Appetite.

[29]  C. Rock,et al.  Caretaker-child interaction during children's 24-hour dietary recalls: who contributes what to the recall record? , 2000, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[30]  M. Livingstone,et al.  Energy requirements and dietary energy recommendations for children and adolescents 1 to 18 years old. , 1996, European journal of clinical nutrition.

[31]  J. Warren,et al.  How well do children aged 5–7 years recall food eaten at school lunch? , 2003, Public Health Nutrition.

[32]  M. Livingstone,et al.  Markers of the validity of reported energy intake. , 2003, The Journal of nutrition.

[33]  M. Goran,et al.  Comparison of energy intake by semiquantitative food-frequency questionnaire with total energy expenditure by the doubly labeled water method in young children. , 1994, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[34]  J. Twisk,et al.  Longitudinal development and tracking of calcium and dairy intake from teenager to adult , 1997, European Journal of Clinical Nutrition.

[35]  W. Mechelen,et al.  Tracking of risk factors for coronary heart disease over a 14-year period: a comparison between lifestyle and biologic risk factors with data from the Amsterdam Growth and Health Study. , 1997, American journal of epidemiology.

[36]  S. Hui,et al.  Nutrient intake variability in a pediatric population: implications for study design. , 1991, The Journal of nutrition.

[37]  E. Obarzanek,et al.  Comparative advantage of 3-day food records over 24-hour recall and 5-day food frequency validated by observation of 9- and 10-year-old girls. , 1994, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[38]  P. Robson,et al.  Measurement of dietary intake in children , 2000, Proceedings of the Nutrition Society.

[39]  L A Lytle,et al.  Validation of 24-hour recalls assisted by food records in third-grade children. The CATCH Collaborative Group. , 1993, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[40]  Rachel K. Johnson,et al.  Influence of body composition on the accuracy of reported energy intake in children. , 2000, Obesity research.

[41]  B. Gower,et al.  Relationships between dietary fat, body fat, and serum lipid profile in prepubertal children. , 1998, Obesity research.

[42]  D. Bowen,et al.  What did I eat yesterday? Determinants of accuracy in 24‐hour food memories , 1995 .

[43]  T J Cole,et al.  Between- and within-subject variation in nutrient intake from infancy to old age: estimating the number of days required to rank dietary intakes with desired precision. , 1989, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[44]  C. Kuzawa,et al.  Adipose tissue in human infancy and childhood: an evolutionary perspective. , 1998, American journal of physical anthropology.

[45]  R. Vandongen,et al.  A comparison of methods of dietary assessment in Australian children aged 11-12 years. , 1989, European journal of clinical nutrition.

[46]  A F Smith,et al.  How children remember what they have eaten. , 1994, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[47]  K J Ellis,et al.  Pubertal African-American girls expend less energy at rest and during physical activity than Caucasian girls. , 1999, The Journal of clinical endocrinology and metabolism.

[48]  A. Black The sensitivity and specificity of the Goldberg cut-off for EI:BMR for identifying diet reports of poor validity , 2000, European Journal of Clinical Nutrition.

[49]  S. B. Domel,et al.  Self-reports of diet: how children remember what they have eaten. , 1997, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[50]  R. Cockington,et al.  Tracking of serum lipids and dietary energy, fat and calcium intake from 1 to 15 years , 1995, Acta paediatrica.

[51]  T. Baranowski,et al.  Fruit and vegetable food frequencies by fourth and fifth grade students: validity and reliability. , 1994, Journal of the American College of Nutrition.

[52]  D. Turner,et al.  Methods of Dietary Investigation , 1942, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[53]  C. Champagne,et al.  Underreporting of energy intake in biracial children is verified by doubly labeled water. , 1996, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[54]  Rachel K. Johnson,et al.  Comparison of multiple-pass 24-hour recall estimates of energy intake with total energy expenditure determined by the doubly labeled water method in young children. , 1996, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[55]  T. Cole,et al.  Within- and between-subject variation in energy expenditure measured by the doubly-labelled water technique: implications for validating reported dietary energy intake , 2000, European Journal of Clinical Nutrition.

[56]  Thomas N Robinson,et al.  Validity of children's food portion estimates: a comparison of 2 measurement aids. , 2002, Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine.

[57]  M C Moore,et al.  Using graduated food models in taking dietary histories. , 1967, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[58]  B. Lausen,et al.  Energy intake of 1 to 18 year old German children and adolescents. , 1998, Zeitschrift fur Ernahrungswissenschaft.

[59]  AE Black,et al.  Critical evaluation of energy intake using the Goldberg cut-off for energy intake:basal metabolic rate. A practical guide to its calculation, use and limitations , 2000, International Journal of Obesity.

[60]  Susan L. Johnson,et al.  The variability of young children's energy intake. , 1991, The New England journal of medicine.

[61]  J. Reilly,et al.  Energy intake by multiple pass 24 h recall and total energy expenditure: a comparison in a representative sample of 3–4-year-olds , 2001, British Journal of Nutrition.

[62]  P. Robson,et al.  Tracking of nutrient intakes in adolescence: the experiences of the Young Hearts Project, Northern Ireland , 2000, British Journal of Nutrition.

[63]  R. Klesges,et al.  Accuracy of self-reports of food intake in obese and normal-weight individuals: effects of parental obesity on reports of children's dietary intake. , 1988, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[64]  G. Berenson,et al.  Dietary studies of children from a biracial population: intakes of carbohydrate and fiber in 10- and 13-year-olds. , 1985, Journal of the American College of Nutrition.

[65]  G. Samuelson,et al.  An epidemiological study of child health and nutrition in a northern Swedish county. , 1971, Odontologisk revy.

[66]  M. Livingstone,et al.  Energy and fat intake in obese and lean children at varying risk of obesity , 2002, International Journal of Obesity.

[67]  G. Berenson,et al.  Adapting the 24-hr. recall for epidemiologic studies of school children. , 1977, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[68]  C. Basch,et al.  Variability and tracking of nutrient intakes of preschool children based on multiple administrations of the 24-hour dietary recall. , 1991, American journal of epidemiology.

[69]  C. Sharbaugh,et al.  Reliability and validity of the 24-hour recall. , 1981, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[70]  R. Klesges,et al.  Validation of the 24-hour dietary recall in preschool children. , 1987, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[71]  G J Petot,et al.  Validity of portion-size measurement aids: a review. , 1997, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[72]  H. Kemper,et al.  Longitudinal trends in and tracking of energy and nutrient intake over 20 years in a Dutch cohort of men and women between 13 and 33 years of age: The Amsterdam growth and health longitudinal study , 2001, British Journal of Nutrition.

[73]  S A Jebb,et al.  Critical evaluation of energy intake data using fundamental principles of energy physiology: 1. Derivation of cut-off limits to identify under-recording. , 1991, European journal of clinical nutrition.

[74]  T. Baranowski,et al.  Accuracy of maternal dietary recall for preschool children. , 1991, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[75]  A M Prentice,et al.  Human energy expenditure in affluent societies: an analysis of 574 doubly-labelled water measurements. , 1996, European journal of clinical nutrition.

[76]  E Chambers,et al.  Cognitive strategies for reporting portion sizes using dietary recall procedures. , 2000, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[77]  R. Farris,et al.  Dietary assessment in children using electronic methods: telephones and tape recorders. , 1990, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[78]  R. Klesges,et al.  Recall of a child's intake from one meal: are parents accurate? , 1989, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[79]  A. Hill,et al.  A weight on children's minds: body shape dissatisfactions at 9-years old. , 1994, International journal of obesity and related metabolic disorders : journal of the International Association for the Study of Obesity.

[80]  K. A. Coleman,et al.  The problem of memory in nutritional epidemiology research. , 1987, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[81]  How well do school children recall what they have eaten? , 1951, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[82]  T. Baranowski,et al.  A cognitive model of children's reporting of food intake. , 1994, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[83]  Maribeth H. Johnson,et al.  How do you remember you ate...?": a Delphi technique study to identify retrieval categories from fourth-grade children. , 1996 .