Disentangling attention from action in the emotional spatial cueing task

In the emotional spatial cueing task, a peripheral cue—either emotional or non-emotional—is presented before target onset. A stronger cue validity effect with an emotional relative to a non-emotional cue (i.e., more efficient responding to validly cued targets relative to invalidly cued targets) is taken as an indication of emotional modulation of attentional processes. However, results from previous emotional spatial cueing studies are not consistent. Some studies find an effect at the validly cued location (shorter reaction times compared to a non-emotional cue), whereas other studies find an effect at the invalidly cued location (longer reaction times compared to a non-emotional cue). In the current paper, we explore which parameters affect emotional modulation of the cue validity effect in the spatial cueing task. Results from five experiments in healthy volunteers led to the conclusion that a threatening spatial cue did not affect attention processes but rather indicate that motor processes are affected. A possible mechanism might be that a strong aversive cue stimulus decreases reaction times by means of stronger action preparation. Consequently, in case of a spatially congruent response with the peripheral cue, a stronger cue validity effect could be obtained due to stronger response priming. The implications for future research are discussed.

[1]  Antonio Schettino,et al.  rain mechanisms for emotional influences on perception and attention : hat is magic and what is not , 2012 .

[2]  Tobias Egner,et al.  Neural Mechanisms Mediating Contingent Capture of Attention by Affective Stimuli , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  Thomas Schmidt,et al.  Dos and don’ts in response priming research , 2011, Advances in cognitive psychology.

[4]  A. Arntz,et al.  Modulation of responses to emotional information in pathological anxiety , 2011 .

[5]  G. Pourtois,et al.  Additive effects of emotional, endogenous, and exogenous attention: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  J. Kenemans,et al.  Attentional bias in high- and low-anxious individuals: Evidence for threat-induced effects on engagement and disengagement , 2011, Cognition & emotion.

[7]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Signals of threat do not capture, but prioritize, attention: a conditioning approach. , 2011, Emotion.

[8]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Looking out for danger: An attentional bias towards spatially predictable threatening stimuli. , 2010, Behaviour research and therapy.

[9]  J. Theeuwes Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[10]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  Anxiety enhances threat processing without competition among multiple inputs: a diffusion model analysis. , 2010, Emotion.

[11]  W. Fias,et al.  Aversive Conditioning under Conditions of Restricted Awareness: Effects on Spatial Cueing , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[12]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Emotional stimuli modulate readiness for action: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study , 2010, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[13]  J. Yiend The effects of emotion on attention: A review of attentional processing of emotional information , 2010 .

[14]  F. Smulders,et al.  Biased processing of emotional information in sub-clinical anxiety: Evidence from Simon effects , 2009 .

[15]  B. de Gelder,et al.  Orienting to threat: faster localization of fearful facial expressions and body postures revealed by saccadic eye movements , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[16]  Geert Crombez,et al.  Attentional bias to threat: a perceptual accuracy approach. , 2008, Emotion.

[17]  Matthew Garner,et al.  Effects of threat cues on attentional shifting, disengagement and response slowing in anxious individuals , 2008, Behaviour research and therapy.

[18]  S. Hofmann Cognitive processes during fear acquisition and extinction in animals and humans: implications for exposure therapy of anxiety disorders. , 2008, Clinical psychology review.

[19]  Josh M. Cisler,et al.  Phenomenological Characteristics of Attentional Biases Towards Threat: A Critical Review , 2009, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[20]  B. de Gelder,et al.  Rapid detection of fear in body expressions, an ERP study , 2007, Brain Research.

[21]  F. Smulders,et al.  A Simon effect for threat-related stimulus content. , 2007, Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry.

[22]  G. Crombez,et al.  A time-course analysis of attentional cueing by threatening scenes. , 2007, Experimental psychology.

[23]  Y. Bar-Haim,et al.  Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. , 2007, Psychological bulletin.

[24]  Bruno Verschuere,et al.  Components of attentional bias to threat in high trait anxiety: Facilitated engagement, impaired disengagement, and attentional avoidance. , 2006, Behaviour research and therapy.

[25]  G. Crombez,et al.  The role of extinction and reinstatement in attentional bias to threat: a conditioning approach. , 2006, Behaviour research and therapy.

[26]  Gereon R. Fink,et al.  Cue validity modulates the neural correlates of covert endogenous orienting of attention in parietal and frontal cortex , 2006, NeuroImage.

[27]  J. Hyönä,et al.  Eye movement assessment of selective attentional capture by emotional pictures. , 2006, Emotion.

[28]  P. Vuilleumier,et al.  How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  Bruno Verschuere,et al.  Signals for threat modulate attentional capture and holding: Fear-conditioning and extinction during the exogenous cueing task , 2005 .

[30]  G. Crombez,et al.  Does imminent threat capture and hold attention? , 2004, Emotion.

[31]  D. Grandjean,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of rapid spatial orienting towards fearful faces. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[32]  G. Crombez,et al.  Fear-conditioned cues of impending pain facilitate attentional engagement , 2004, Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology.

[33]  A. Mathews,et al.  A Cognitive Model of Selective Processing in Anxiety , 1998, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[34]  D. Simons,et al.  Moving and looming stimuli capture attention , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[35]  Anthony Lambert,et al.  Do isoluminant color changes capture attention? , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[36]  R. Dolan,et al.  Modulation of spatial attention by fear-conditioned stimuli: an event-related fMRI study , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  E. Fox,et al.  Attentional bias for threat: Evidence for delayed disengagement from emotional faces , 2002, Cognition & emotion.

[38]  Robert J Snowden,et al.  Visual Attention to Color: Parvocellular Guidance of Attentional Resources? , 2002, Psychological science.

[39]  E. Fox,et al.  Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[40]  A. Ohman,et al.  Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[41]  A. Mathews,et al.  Anxiety and Attention to Threatening Pictures , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[42]  P. Lang,et al.  Fear and anxiety: animal models and human cognitive psychophysiology. , 2000, Journal of affective disorders.

[43]  J. Tipples,et al.  Orienting to exogenous cues and attentional bias to affective pictures reflect separate processes. , 2000, British journal of psychology.

[44]  Kenneth Hugdahl,et al.  Cognition and the autonomic nervous system: Orienting, anticipation, and conditioning. , 2000 .

[45]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Handbook of psychophysiology (2nd ed.). , 2000 .

[46]  Kenneth Hugdahl,et al.  Emotional modulation of attention orienting: A classical conditioning study , 1999 .

[47]  G. Crombez,et al.  Extinction in fear conditioning: effects on startle modulation and evaluative self-reports. , 1998, Psychophysiology.

[48]  K. Hugdahl,et al.  Peripheral cuing of covert spatial attention before and after emotional conditioning of the cue. , 1996, The International journal of neuroscience.

[49]  M Eimer,et al.  Stimulus-response compatibility and automatic response activation: evidence from psychophysiological studies. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[50]  J. Theeuwes Abrupt luminance change pops out; abrupt color change does not , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[51]  R. Proctor,et al.  The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[52]  P. Lang The emotion probe. Studies of motivation and attention. , 1995, The American psychologist.

[53]  S. Luck,et al.  The role of attention in feature detection and conjunction discrimination: an electrophysiological analysis. , 1995, The International journal of neuroscience.

[54]  J. Theeuwes Endogenous and Exogenous Control of Visual Selection , 1994, Perception.

[55]  M Eimer,et al.  Spatial cueing, sensory gating and selective response preparation: an ERP study on visuo-spatial orienting. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[56]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[57]  J. Theeuwes Perceptual selectivity for color and form , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[58]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[59]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[60]  C. MacLeod,et al.  Attentional bias in emotional disorders. , 1986, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[61]  Arnulf Remole,et al.  VISUAL MASKING: AN INTEGRATIVE APPROACH , 1985 .

[62]  M. Posner,et al.  Components of visual orienting , 1984 .

[63]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[64]  A. Dickinson Conditioning and associative learning. , 1981, British medical bulletin.

[65]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[66]  N. Mackintosh The psychology of animal learning , 1974 .

[67]  J. R. Simon,et al.  Auditory S-R compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. , 1967, The Journal of applied psychology.