Bottom-up influences on voluntary task switching: the elusive homunculus escapes.

Voluntary action can be studied by giving participants free choice over which task to perform in response to each presented stimulus. In such experiments, performance costs are observed when participants choose to switch tasks from the previous trial. It has been proposed that these costs primarily index the time-consuming operation of top-down control processes that support voluntary action. The present experiments showed, contrary to this view, that greater costs were associated with voluntary switching to the easier task of a pair. These increased switch costs for the easier task were accompanied by a reliable preference of the participants for performing the other, more difficult task. Interference between tasks during response selection was identified as the critical factor driving these effects of task difficulty. Together, the findings suggest that participants' voluntary choices, and the time taken to execute those choices, may not directly index the operation of cognitive control but instead may reflect complex interactions between top-down and bottom-up influences on behavior.

[1]  Ulrich Mayr,et al.  Task selection cost asymmetry without task switching , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[2]  A. Jersild Mental set and shift , 2011 .

[3]  A. Treisman,et al.  Attention, Space, and Action: Studies in Cognitive Neuroscience , 2001 .

[4]  M. Brass,et al.  Voluntary Selection of Task Sets Revealed by Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2006 .

[5]  Ritske de Jong,et al.  Strategical determinants of compatibility effects with task uncertainty , 1995 .

[6]  E. M. Altmann,et al.  Cue-independent task-specific representations in task switching: evidence from backward inhibition. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[7]  Catherine M. Arrington,et al.  The role of attentional networks in voluntary task switching , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[8]  Robert W. Proctor,et al.  Repetition effects with categorizable stimulus and response sets. , 1993 .

[9]  S. Sternberg The discovery of processing stages , 1969 .

[10]  Catherine M. Arrington,et al.  Perceptual asymmetries influence task choice: The effect of lateralised presentation of hierarchical stimuli , 2010, Laterality.

[11]  V. Ferreira,et al.  Should I stay or should I switch? A cost-benefit analysis of voluntary language switching in young and aging bilinguals. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[12]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Jumping the gun: is effective preparation contingent upon anticipatory activation in task-relevant neural circuitry? , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[13]  E Ruthruff,et al.  Switching between simple cognitive tasks: the interaction of top-down and bottom-up factors. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  The effects of recent practice on task switching. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  Catherine M Arrington,et al.  The effect of stimulus availability on task choice in voluntary task switching , 2008, Memory & cognition.

[16]  Catherine M Arrington,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article The Cost of a Voluntary Task Switch , 2022 .

[17]  G. Schwartz,et al.  Consciousness and Self-Regulation , 1976 .

[18]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[19]  Saul Sternberg,et al.  The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method , 1969 .

[20]  N. Meiran Modeling cognitive control in task-switching , 2000, Psychological research.

[21]  D. Alan Allport,et al.  SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .

[22]  Carolin Dudschig,et al.  Short Article: Why do we slow down after an error? Mechanisms underlying the effects of posterror slowing , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[23]  G. Logan,et al.  Clever homunculus: is there an endogenous act of control in the explicit task-cuing procedure? , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  R De Jong,et al.  An intention-activation account of residual switch costs , 2000 .

[25]  Ronald Hübner,et al.  Effects of stimulus features and instruction on response coding, selection, and inhibition: Evidence from repetition effects under task switching , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[26]  N. Meiran Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. , 1996 .

[27]  G. Logan Executive control of thought and action , 1985 .

[28]  A. Allport,et al.  Cue-based preparation and stimulus-based priming of tasks in task switching , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[29]  Eric Ruthruff,et al.  Inhibition of task set: Converging evidence from task choice in the voluntary task-switching paradigm , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[30]  A. Osman,et al.  Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.

[31]  G. Wylie,et al.  Task-switching: Positive and negative priming of task-set. , 1999 .

[32]  B. Hommel,et al.  Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus–task bindings in task-shift costs , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.

[33]  Harold Pashler,et al.  Procedural learning: II. Intertrial repetition effects in speeded-choice tasks. , 1991 .

[34]  Ulrich Mayr,et al.  On How to be Unpredictable : Evidence from the Voluntary Task-Switching Paradigm , 2005 .

[35]  M. Schmitter-Edgecombe,et al.  Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks following severe closed-head injury. , 2006, Neuropsychology.

[36]  S. Keele,et al.  Changing internal constraints on action: the role of backward inhibition. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[37]  A. Allport,et al.  Bilingual Language Switching in Naming: Asymmetrical Costs of Language Selection , 1999 .

[38]  R. Woodworth Archives of psychology , 2010 .

[39]  Catherine M Arrington,et al.  Voluntary task switching: chasing the elusive homunculus. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[40]  D. Norman,et al.  Attention to Action: Willed and Automatic Control of Behavior Technical Report No. 8006. , 1980 .

[41]  Jelle Demanet,et al.  Short Article: Is Advance Reconfiguration in Voluntary Task Switching Affected by the Design Employed? , 2009 .

[42]  E. Geller,et al.  Stimulus anticipation in choice reaction time with variable S-R mapping , 1972 .

[43]  Matthew M. Botvinick,et al.  Anticipation of cognitive demand during decision-making , 2009, Psychological research.

[44]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Between-Task Competition and Cognitive Control in Task Switching , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  J. Driver,et al.  Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , 2000 .

[46]  N. Yeung,et al.  Switching between tasks of unequal familiarity: the role of stimulus-attribute and response-set selection. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[47]  T. Shallice,et al.  Task Switching : A PDP Model , 2001 .

[48]  D. Meyer,et al.  Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[49]  A. Allport,et al.  Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs” , 2000, Psychological research.

[50]  Shaffer Lh CHOICE REACTION WITH VARIABLE S-R MAPPING. , 1965 .

[51]  D. Norman,et al.  Attention to action: Willed and automatic control , 1980 .

[52]  J R Anderson,et al.  Task preparation and task repetition: two-component model of task switching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[53]  Andrea M Philipp,et al.  Response selection and response execution in task switching: evidence from a go-signal paradigm. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.