Depth cues, behavioural context, and natural illumination: some potential limitations of video playback techniques
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] P. Green,et al. HEAD-BOBBING DURING WALKING, RUNNING AND FLYING: RELATIVE MOTION PERCEPTION IN THE PIGEON , 1988 .
[2] Able. The flexible migratory orientation system of the savannah sparrow (Passerculus sandwichensis) , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.
[3] P. Marler,et al. Effects of apparent size and speed on the response of chickens, Gallus gallus, to computer-generated simulations of aerial predators , 1993, Animal Behaviour.
[4] Michael F. Land. Polarizing the world of fish , 1991, Nature.
[5] G. Rosenthal,et al. Female preference for swords in Xiphophorus helleri reflects a bias for large apparent size. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[6] Johannes M. Zanker,et al. A glimpse into crabworld , 1997, Vision Research.
[7] I. Walker. Habituation to disturbance in the fiddler crab (Uca annulipes) in its natural environment , 1972 .
[8] Vision and near orientation of fish , 1970 .
[9] E. Denton,et al. Bands against stripes on the backs of mackerel, Scomber scombrus L. , 1998, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[10] W. Kirmse,et al. Binocular vision and accommodation in prey-catching chameleons , 1998, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.
[11] N Shashar,et al. Polarization contrast vision in Octopus. , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.
[12] H. Bülthoff,et al. Does the brain know the physics of specular reflection? , 1990, Nature.
[13] R. Wehner. The Hymenopteran Skylight Compass: Matched Filtering and Parallel Coding , 1989 .
[14] E. Pugh,et al. Double cones as a basis for a new type of polarization vision in vertebrates , 1991, Nature.
[15] John D. Pettigrew,et al. Convergence of specialised behaviour, eye movements and visual optics in the sandlance (Teleostei) and the chameleon (Reptilia) , 1999, Current Biology.
[16] W. Hershberger,et al. Attached-shadow orientation perceived as depth by chickens reared in an environment illuminated from below. , 1970, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[17] M. Dawkins,et al. Distance and the presentation of visual stimuli to birds , 1997, Animal Behaviour.
[18] B. Frost. The Optokinetic Basis of Head-bobbing in the Pigeon , 1978 .
[19] J. Endler,et al. Some comments on visual perception and the use of video playback in animal behavior studies , 2000, acta ethologica.
[20] The physical basis of reflective communication between fish, with special reference to the horse mackerel, Trachurus trachurus , 1997 .
[21] V. S. Ramachandran,et al. Perception of shape from shading , 1988, Nature.
[22] M. Heisenberg,et al. The sensory-motor link in motion-dependent flight control of flies. , 1993, Reviews of oculomotor research.
[23] T. Cronin,et al. Polarization vision in cuttlefish in a concealed communication channel? , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.
[24] J B Deregowski,et al. Pictorial perception and culture. , 1972, Scientific American.
[25] J. Zeil. Homing in fiddler crabs (Uca lactea annulipes and Uca vomeris : Ocypodidae) , 1998, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.
[26] Sally A. McFadden,et al. Binocular Depth Perception , 1994 .
[27] Richard B. D’Eath,et al. Can video images imitate real stimuli in animal behaviour experiments? , 1998 .
[28] F. Schaeffel. Functional Accommodation in Birds , 1994 .
[29] S. McKee,et al. The imprecision of stereopsis , 1990, Vision Research.
[30] N. Shashar,et al. Cuttlefish use polarization sensitivity in predation on silvery fish , 2000, Vision Research.
[31] Patrick R. Green,et al. Multiple Sources of Depth Information: An Ecological Approach , 1994 .
[32] LEO J FLEISHMAN,et al. Colour perception and the use of video playback experiments in animal behaviour , 1998, Animal Behaviour.
[33] M. Land,et al. Fiddler Crabs Use the Visual Horizon to Distinguish Predators from Conspecifics: A Review of the Evidence , 1997, Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom.