Individual preferences modulate incentive values: Evidence from functional MRI

BackgroundIn most studies on human reward processing, reward intensity has been manipulated on an objective scale (e.g., varying monetary value). Everyday experience, however, teaches us that objectively equivalent rewards may differ substantially in their subjective incentive values. One factor influencing incentive value in humans is branding. The current study explores the hypothesis that individual brand preferences modulate activity in reward areas similarly to objectively measurable differences in reward intensity.MethodsA wheel-of-fortune game comprising an anticipation phase and a subsequent outcome evaluation phase was implemented. Inside a 3 Tesla MRI scanner, 19 participants played for chocolate bars of three different brands that differed in subjective attractiveness.ResultsParametrical analysis of the obtained fMRI data demonstrated that the level of activity in anatomically distinct neural networks was linearly associated with the subjective preference hierarchy of the brands played for. During the anticipation phases, preference-dependent neural activity has been registered in premotor areas, insular cortex, orbitofrontal cortex, and in the midbrain. During the outcome phases, neural activity in the caudate nucleus, precuneus, lingual gyrus, cerebellum, and in the pallidum was influenced by individual preference.ConclusionOur results suggest a graded effect of differently preferred brands onto the incentive value of objectively equivalent rewards. Regarding the anticipation phase, the results reflect an intensified state of wanting that facilitates action preparation when the participants play for their favorite brand. This mechanism may underlie approach behavior in real-life choice situations.

[1]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Characterizing Stimulus–Response Functions Using Nonlinear Regressors in Parametric fMRI Experiments , 1998, NeuroImage.

[2]  W. Schultz Multiple reward signals in the brain , 2000, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[3]  P. Boesiger,et al.  SENSE: Sensitivity encoding for fast MRI , 1999, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[4]  Brian Knutson,et al.  Dissociation of reward anticipation and outcome with event-related fMRI , 2001, Neuroreport.

[5]  E. Rolls,et al.  Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[6]  Hilke Plassmann,et al.  Nonlinear Responses Within the Medial Prefrontal Cortex Reveal When Specific Implicit Information Influences Economic Decision Making , 2005, Journal of neuroimaging : official journal of the American Society of Neuroimaging.

[7]  J. Schmahmann Disorders of the cerebellum: ataxia, dysmetria of thought, and the cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. , 2004, The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[8]  P. Glimcher,et al.  The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  Markus Junghöfer,et al.  Selective Visual Attention to Emotion , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  S. Hyman,et al.  Acute Effects of Cocaine on Human Brain Activity and Emotion , 1997, Neuron.

[11]  A. Graybiel,et al.  Effect of the nigrostriatal dopamine system on acquired neural responses in the striatum of behaving monkeys. , 1994, Science.

[12]  R. Elliott,et al.  Differential Response Patterns in the Striatum and Orbitofrontal Cortex to Financial Reward in Humans: A Parametric Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  ‘What’ and ‘where’ in the human brain , 1994, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[14]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Functional Imaging of Neural Responses to Expectancy and Experience of Monetary Gains and Losses tasks with monetary payoffs , 2001 .

[15]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dissociable Neural Responses in Human Reward Systems , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  K. Doya,et al.  A Neural Correlate of Reward-Based Behavioral Learning in Caudate Nucleus: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of a Stochastic Decision Task , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  Jeffrey C. Cooper,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging of reward prediction , 2005, Current opinion in neurology.

[18]  Michael Rotte,et al.  Favorite brands as cultural objects modulate reward circuit , 2007, Neuroreport.

[19]  N. Tzourio-Mazoyer,et al.  Automated Anatomical Labeling of Activations in SPM Using a Macroscopic Anatomical Parcellation of the MNI MRI Single-Subject Brain , 2002, NeuroImage.

[20]  K. Berridge,et al.  Erratum to: “Parsing reward” [Trends Neurosci. 26 (2003) 507–513] , 2003, Trends in Neurosciences.

[21]  M. Roesch,et al.  Neuronal Activity Related to Reward Value and Motivation in Primate Frontal Cortex , 2004, Science.

[22]  H. Critchley,et al.  Neural Activity Relating to Generation and Representation of Galvanic Skin Conductance Responses: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  Michael Rotte,et al.  Thinking on luxury or pragmatic brand products: Brain responses to different categories of culturally based brands , 2007, Brain Research.

[24]  G. Mangun,et al.  The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[25]  Kent C. Berridge,et al.  Pleasures of the brain , 2003, Brain and Cognition.

[26]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  Neural Correlates of Behavioral Preference for Culturally Familiar Drinks , 2004, Neuron.

[27]  Lutz Jäncke,et al.  Neural correlates of a ‘pessimistic’ attitude when anticipating events of unknown emotional valence , 2007, NeuroImage.

[28]  A. Sanfey,et al.  Models and Methods in Delay Discounting , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[29]  Harald T. Schupp,et al.  Emotional Facilitation of Sensory Processing in the Visual Cortex , 2003, Psychological science.

[30]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Regret and its avoidance: a neuroimaging study of choice behavior , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  K. Berridge,et al.  Parsing reward , 2003, Trends in Neurosciences.

[32]  M. Junghöfer,et al.  Emotion and attention: event-related brain potential studies. , 2006, Progress in brain research.

[33]  K. Berridge Food reward: Brain substrates of wanting and liking , 1996, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[34]  R. Sugden,et al.  Regret Theory: An alternative theory of rational choice under uncertainty Review of Economic Studies , 1982 .

[35]  H. Critchley,et al.  Cerebral correlates of autonomic cardiovascular arousal: a functional neuroimaging investigation in humans , 2000, The Journal of physiology.

[36]  E. Rolls,et al.  Hunger and satiety modify the responses of olfactory and visual neurons in the primate orbitofrontal cortex. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  H. Damasio,et al.  The Iowa Gambling Task and the somatic marker hypothesis: some questions and answers , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[38]  D. Premack Reversibility of the Reinforcement Relation , 1962, Science.

[39]  R. Dolan,et al.  Brain, emotion and decision making: the paradigmatic example of regret , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[40]  E. Rolls,et al.  Orbitofrontal cortex neurons: role in olfactory and visual association learning. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Mapping Motor Inhibition: Conjunctive Brain Activations across Different Versions of Go/No-Go and Stop Tasks , 2001, NeuroImage.

[42]  Brian Knutson,et al.  A region of mesial prefrontal cortex tracks monetarily rewarding outcomes: characterization with rapid event-related fMRI , 2003, NeuroImage.

[43]  J. A. Frost,et al.  Conceptual Processing during the Conscious Resting State: A Functional MRI Study , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[44]  M. Mesulam,et al.  Dissociation of Neural Representation of Intensity and Affective Valuation in Human Gustation , 2003, Neuron.

[45]  J. Binder,et al.  A Parametric Manipulation of Factors Affecting Task-induced Deactivation in Functional Neuroimaging , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[46]  B. Mazoyer,et al.  Cortical networks for working memory and executive functions sustain the conscious resting state in man , 2001, Brain Research Bulletin.

[47]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Neural Responses during Anticipation of a Primary Taste Reward , 2002, Neuron.

[48]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerebral activation , 1996, Human brain mapping.

[49]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Common Blood Flow Changes across Visual Tasks: II. Decreases in Cerebral Cortex , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[50]  A. Sirigu,et al.  The Involvement of the Orbitofrontal Cortex in the Experience of Regret , 2004, Science.

[51]  A. Kacelnik Normative and descriptive models of decision making: time discounting and risk sensitivity. , 2007, Ciba Foundation symposium.

[52]  K. Berman,et al.  Cerebral Cortex doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj004 Neural Coding of Distinct Statistical Properties of Reward Information in Humans , 2005 .

[53]  N. Alpert,et al.  Neural activation during sexual and competitive arousal in healthy men , 1999, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.

[54]  R. E. Passingham,et al.  Prediction error for free monetary reward in the human prefrontal cortex , 2004, NeuroImage.

[55]  E. Rolls,et al.  Cerebral Cortex Advance Access published June 22, 2007 Expected Value, Reward Outcome, and Temporal Difference Error Representations in a Probabilistic Decision Task , 2022 .

[56]  C. I. Connolly,et al.  Building neural representations of habits. , 1999, Science.

[57]  Toshikazu Hasegawa,et al.  A hyperbolic decay of subjective probability of obtaining delayed rewards , 2007, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[58]  Daeyeol Lee,et al.  Prefrontal Coding of Temporally Discounted Values during Intertemporal Choice , 2008, Neuron.

[59]  Kyle S. Smith,et al.  Ventral pallidum firing codes hedonic reward: when a bad taste turns good. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.