Perceptual effects of unequal saccadic adaptation produced by a dichoptic step

The binocular coordination of eye movements in a three-dimensional environment involves a combination of saccade and vergence movements. To maintain binocular accuracy and control in the face of sensory and motor changes (that occur with e.g., normal aging, surgery, corrective lenses), the oculomotor system must adapt in response to manifest visual errors. This may be achieved through a combination of binocular and monocular mechanisms, including the recalibration of saccade and vergence amplitudes in response to different visual errors induced in each eye (Maiello, Harrison, & Bex, 2016). This work has used a double-step paradigm to recalibrate eye movements in response to visual errors produced by dichoptic target steps (e.g., leftward in the left eye and rightward in the right eye). Here, we evaluated the immediate perceptual effects of this adaptation. Experiment 1 measured localization errors following adaptation by comparing the apparent locations of pre- and postsaccadic probes. Consistent with previous work showing localization errors following saccadic adaptation, our results demonstrated that adaptation to a dichoptic step produces different localization errors in the two eyes. Furthermore, in Experiment 2, this effect was reduced for a vergence shift in the absence of a saccade, indicating that saccade programming is responsible for a large component of this illusory shift. Experiment 3 measured postsaccadic stereopsis thresholds and indicated that, unlike localization judgments, adaptation did not influence stereoacuity. Together, these results demonstrate novel dichoptic visual errors following oculomotor adaptation and point to monocular and binocular mechanisms involved in the maintenance of binocular coordination.

[1]  C Busettini,et al.  Short-term saccadic adaptation in the macaque monkey: a binocular mechanism. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  Peter J. Bex,et al.  Monocular and Binocular Contributions to Oculomotor Plasticity , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[3]  S. McKee,et al.  The imprecision of stereopsis , 1990, Vision Research.

[4]  Heiner Deubel,et al.  Adaptive Control of Saccade Metrics , 1991 .

[5]  Olivier A Coubard,et al.  Saccade and vergence eye movements: a review of motor and premotor commands , 2013, The European journal of neuroscience.

[6]  C. Blakemore The range and scope of binocular depth discrimination in man , 1970, The Journal of physiology.

[7]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[8]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Binocular co‐ordination of human horizontal saccadic eye movements. , 1988, The Journal of physiology.

[9]  Yanning H. Han,et al.  Properties of anticipatory vergence responses. , 2002, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[10]  Roy Gordon Cole,et al.  EFFECT OF FIXATION DISPARITY ON STEREO‐ACUITY* , 1974 .

[11]  R S Harwerth,et al.  Viewing Time and Stereoscopic Threshold with Random‐Dot Stereograms , 1977, American journal of optometry and physiological optics.

[12]  M. Kenward,et al.  An Introduction to the Bootstrap , 2007 .

[13]  T Anstis,et al.  Saccadic plasticity: parametric adaptive control by retinal feedback. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  S Krekling STEREOSCOPIC THRESHOLD WITHIN THE STEREOSCOPIC RANGE IN CENTRAL VISION* , 1974, American journal of optometry and physiological optics.

[15]  G. Westheimer,et al.  Disjunctive eye movements , 1961, The Journal of physiology.

[16]  D. Pélisson,et al.  Sensorimotor adaptation of saccadic eye movements , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[17]  J. E. Albano,et al.  Binocular interactions in rapid saccadic adaptation , 1995, Vision Research.

[18]  S. C. Mclaughlin Parametric adjustment in saccadic eye movements , 1967 .

[19]  David C. Burr,et al.  Compression of visual space before saccades , 1997, Nature.

[20]  Bruce Bridgeman,et al.  Failure to detect displacement of the visual world during saccadic eye movements , 1975, Vision Research.

[21]  Dave M. Stampe,et al.  Heuristic filtering and reliable calibration methods for video-based pupil-tracking systems , 1993 .

[22]  E Kowler,et al.  Illusory shifts in visual direction accompany adaptation of saccadic eye movements , 1999, Nature.

[23]  J. A. Gisbergen,et al.  Specificity of saccadic adaptation in three-dimensional space , 1997, Vision Research.

[24]  C. Scudder,et al.  Comparison of two methods of producing adaptation of saccade size and implications for the site of plasticity. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  Jeremy R. Cooperstock,et al.  Stereo Vision: The Haves and Have-Nots , 2015, i-Perception.

[26]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Microsaccades are triggered by low retinal image slip. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[27]  Maria Concetta Morrone,et al.  Influence of saccadic adaptation on spatial localization: comparison of verbal and pointing reports. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[28]  Denis Pélisson,et al.  Behavioral evidence of separate adaptation mechanisms controlling saccade amplitude lengthening and shortening. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  Harold E. Bedell,et al.  Changes in oculocentric visual direction induced by the recalibration of saccades , 1988, Vision Research.

[30]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Cortical control of saccades and fixation in man. A PET study. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  R M Steinman,et al.  Asymmetrical adaptation of human saccades to anisometropic spectacles. , 1989, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[32]  K N OGLE,et al.  Stereoscopic vision and the duration of the stimulus. , 1958, A.M.A. archives of ophthalmology.

[33]  F C VOLKMANN,et al.  Vision during voluntary saccadic eye movements. , 1962, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[34]  H Deubel,et al.  Adaptive gain control of saccadic eye movements. , 1986, Human neurobiology.

[35]  Heiner Deubel,et al.  Post-saccadic location judgments reveal remapping of saccade targets to non-foveal locations. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[36]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[37]  K. N. Ogle Precision and validity of stereoscopic depth perception from double images. , 1953, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[38]  Frans W Cornelissen,et al.  The Eyelink Toolbox: Eye tracking with MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox , 2002, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[39]  W. King,et al.  Binocular coordination of eye movements – Hering’s Law of equal innervation or uniocular control? , 2011, The European journal of neuroscience.

[40]  M. T. Bergen,et al.  Short-term predictive changes in the dynamics of disparity vergence eye movements. , 2005, Journal of vision.

[41]  M. Lappe,et al.  Effects of saccadic adaptation on visual localization before and during saccades , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  M. Raichle,et al.  The role of cerebral cortex in the generation of voluntary saccades: a positron emission tomographic study. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  CASPER J ERKELENS,et al.  Trajectories of the Human Binocular Fixation Point during Conjugate and Non-conjugate Gaze-shifts , 1997, Vision Research.

[44]  Harold E. Bedell,et al.  Stereopsis is perturbed by vergence error , 2003, Vision Research.

[45]  J. Semmlow,et al.  The influence of repetitive eye movements on vergence performance , 2000, Vision Research.

[46]  T Usui,et al.  Adaptive changes in dynamic properties of human disparity-induced vergence. , 2001, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[47]  C. Kemner,et al.  Spatial Frequency Training Modulates Neural Face Processing: Learning Transfers from Low- to High-Level Visual Features , 2017, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[48]  Markus Lappe,et al.  Effect of saccadic adaptation on localization of visual targets. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[49]  A. Fuchs,et al.  The characteristics and neuronal substrate of saccadic eye movement plasticity , 2004, Progress in Neurobiology.

[50]  J. Weesie,et al.  Integration of visual and inertial cues in perceived heading of self-motion. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[51]  Arien Mack,et al.  Adaptation to an altered relation between retinal image displacements and saccadic eye movements , 1978, Vision Research.

[52]  J. Demer,et al.  Effect of aging on human rectus extraocular muscle paths demonstrated by magnetic resonance imaging. , 2002, American journal of ophthalmology.

[53]  H. G. Lemij,et al.  Long-term nonconjugate adaptation of human saccades to anisometropic spectacles , 1991, Vision Research.

[54]  Denis Pélisson,et al.  Oculomotor plasticity: Are mechanisms of adaptation for reactive and voluntary saccades separate? , 2007, Brain Research.

[55]  Richard F. Lewis,et al.  Disconjugate adaptation of saccades: contribution of binocular and monocular mechanisms , 1999, Vision Research.

[56]  M. Lappe,et al.  Motor signals in visual localization. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[57]  Robert B Shaw,et al.  Aging of the Facial Skeleton: Aesthetic Implications and Rejuvenation Strategies , 2011, Plastic and reconstructive surgery.

[58]  Jiawei Zhou,et al.  Straightening the Eyes Doesn't Rebalance the Brain , 2017, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[59]  Denis G. Pelli,et al.  ECVP '07 Abstracts , 2007, Perception.