Visual jitter: evidence for visual-motion-based compensation of retinal slip due to small eye movements

When dynamic random noise is replaced by static noise after a period of adaptation, adjacent unadapted regions filled with static noise appear to 'jitter' coherently in random directions for several seconds, actually mirroring the observer's own eye movements of fixation [Murakami, I. & Cavanagh, P. (1998). Nature, 395, 798-801]. The present study aims at psychophysically locating two distinct stages underlying this visual jitter phenomenon: a monocular, adaptable stage that measures local retinal motion and a compensation stage that estimates a baseline motion minimum and subtracts it from motion vectors nearby. The first three experiments revealed that visual jitter has storage, directional selectivity, and spatial frequency selectivity, like the motion after-effect does. These results suggest some overlap in the adaptation mechanisms for the two effects, possibly at or below the level of primary visual cortex. The next two experiments revealed the transfer of the effect across the vertical meridian as well as the existence of a preferred stimulus size that is a linear increasing function of eccentricity, mimicking the RF size of the monkey MT neurons. These results suggest that some extrastriate motion area along the parietal pathway including MT mediates motion-based compensation of retinal slip.

[1]  G. Edelman,et al.  Signal and sense : local and global order in perceptual maps , 1990 .

[2]  C. Gross,et al.  Visual topography of V2 in the macaque , 1981, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[3]  Erratum: Microsaccadic eye movements and firing of single cells in the striate cortex of macaque monkeys , 2000, Nature neuroscience.

[4]  H. Barlow,et al.  Inter-ocular Transfer of Movement After-effects during Pressure Blinding of the Stimulated Eye , 1963, Nature.

[5]  S. Shimojo,et al.  Assimilation-type and Contrast-type Bias of Motion Induced by the Surround in a Random-dot Display: Evidence for Center-Surround Antagonism , 1996, Vision Research.

[6]  D. Burr,et al.  Receptive field size of human motion detection units , 1987, Vision Research.

[7]  G. Orban,et al.  Laminar analysis of motion information processing in macaque V5 , 1989, Brain Research.

[8]  S. Klein,et al.  Detection and discrimination of the direction of motion in central and peripheral vision of normal and amblyopic observers , 1984, Vision Research.

[9]  K. Tanaka,et al.  Analysis of local and wide-field movements in the superior temporal visual areas of the macaque monkey , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[10]  S. McKee,et al.  The detection of motion in the peripheral visual field , 1984, Vision Research.

[11]  DH Hubel,et al.  Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depth , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[12]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Temporal and Spatial Frequency Tuning of the Flicker Motion Aftereffect , 1996, Vision Research.

[13]  S. Shimojo,et al.  Modulation of motion aftereffect by surround motion and its dependence on stimulus size and eccentricity , 1995, Vision Research.

[14]  A. A. Skavenski,et al.  Miniature eye movement. , 1973, Science.

[15]  I. M. Spigel The Effects of Differential Post-Exposure Illumination on the Decay of a Movement After-Effect , 1960 .

[16]  Moshe Gur PII: S0042-6989(96)00182-4 , 1997 .

[17]  R. Blake,et al.  The neural site of binocular rivalry relative to the analysis of motion in the human visual system , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[18]  R. Andersen,et al.  Functional analysis of human MT and related visual cortical areas using magnetic resonance imaging , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[19]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Recovery from adaptation for dynamic and static motion aftereffects: Evidence for two mechanisms , 1996, Vision Research.

[20]  K. Tanaka,et al.  Analysis of object motion in the ventral part of the medial superior temporal area of the macaque visual cortex. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  P. Cz. Handbuch der physiologischen Optik , 1896 .

[22]  R Blake,et al.  Binocular rivalry suppression disrupts recovery from motion adaptation , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.

[23]  A. L. I︠A︡rbus Eye Movements and Vision , 1967 .

[24]  S. Nishida,et al.  Positive motion after-effect induced by bandpass-filtered random-dot kinematograms , 1992, Vision Research.

[25]  Ulker Tulunay-Keesey,et al.  The role of eye movements in motion detection , 1987, Vision Research.

[26]  R A Andersen,et al.  Influence of gaze rotation on the visual response of primate MSTd neurons. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  Jeff B. Pelz,et al.  The role of exocentric reference frames in the perception of visual direction , 1995, Vision Research.

[28]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  The pattern of interhemispheric connections and its relationship to extrastriate visual areas in the macaque monkey , 1982, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[29]  P. Thompson,et al.  The Role of Intervening Patterns in the Storage of the Movement Aftereffect , 1994, Perception.

[30]  G. Orban,et al.  Shape and Spatial Distribution of Receptive Fields and Antagonistic Motion Surrounds in the Middle Temporal Area (V5) of the Macaque , 1995, The European journal of neuroscience.

[31]  P. Thier,et al.  Inability of Rhesus Monkey Area V1 to Discriminate Between Self‐induced and Externally Induced Retinal Image Slip , 1996, The European journal of neuroscience.

[32]  K. H. Britten,et al.  Neuronal correlates of a perceptual decision , 1989, Nature.

[33]  I. Murakami,et al.  Motion aftereffect after monocular adaptation to filled-in motion at the blind spot , 1995, Vision Research.

[34]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[35]  L A RIGGS,et al.  Motions of the retinal image during fixation. , 1954, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[36]  R P Power Apparent Movement Induced by Afterimages , 1983, Perception.

[37]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Response to motion in extrastriate area MSTl: center-surround interactions. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  M. J. Wright,et al.  Matching velocity in central and peripheral vision , 1986, Vision Research.

[39]  C. Gross,et al.  Visuotopic organization and extent of V3 and V4 of the macaque , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[40]  I. M. Spigel Contour absence as a critical factor in the inhibition of the decay of a movement aftereffect. , 1962 .

[41]  C. Gross,et al.  Visual topography of striate projection zone (MT) in posterior superior temporal sulcus of the macaque. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  R. W. Ditchburn,et al.  Assembled data in eye movements. , 1967, Optica acta.

[43]  Nikos K. Logothetis,et al.  Microsaccades differentially modulate neural activity in the striate and extrastriate visual cortex , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[44]  D. Snodderly,et al.  Response Variability of Neurons in Primary Visual Cortex (V1) of Alert Monkeys , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  Shin'ya Nishida,et al.  Linking lower and higher stages of motion processing? , 1997, Vision Research.

[46]  H. Komatsu,et al.  Relation of cortical areas MT and MST to pursuit eye movements. III. Interaction with full-field visual stimulation. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[47]  C. Baker,et al.  Spatial frequency selective mechanisms underlying the motion aftereffect , 1992, Vision Research.

[48]  R Over,et al.  Spatial determinants of the aftereffect of seen motion. , 1973, Vision research.

[49]  J. Allman,et al.  Stimulus specific responses from beyond the classical receptive field: neurophysiological mechanisms for local-global comparisons in visual neurons. , 1985, Annual review of neuroscience.

[50]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Recovery from motion adaptation is delayed by successively presented orthogonal motion , 1994, Vision Research.

[51]  A. Murat Tekalp,et al.  A new motion-compensated reduced-order model Kalman filter for space-varying restoration of progressive and interlaced video , 1998, IEEE Trans. Image Process..

[52]  G. Orban,et al.  Size and shape of receptive fields in the medial superior temporal area (MST) of the macaque , 1997, Neuroreport.

[53]  D. Burr,et al.  Large receptive fields for optic flow detection in humans , 1998, Vision Research.

[54]  O. Grüsser,et al.  Afterimage movement during saccades in the dark , 1987, Vision Research.

[55]  J. M. Hupé,et al.  Cortical feedback improves discrimination between figure and background by V1, V2 and V3 neurons , 1998, Nature.

[56]  A. A. Skavenski,et al.  Quality of retinal image stabilization during small natural and artificial body rotations in man , 1979, Vision Research.

[57]  L. Riggs,et al.  The disappearance of steadily fixated visual test objects. , 1953, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[58]  T. Wiesel,et al.  Relationships between horizontal interactions and functional architecture in cat striate cortex as revealed by cross-correlation analysis , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[59]  I. M. Spigel THE USE OF DECAY INHIBITION IN AN EXAMINATION OF CENTRAL MEDIATION IN MOVEMENT AFTEREFFECTS. , 1964, The Journal of general psychology.

[60]  D. B. Bender,et al.  Selectivity for relative motion in the monkey superior colliculus. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[61]  L. P. O'Keefe,et al.  The influence of fixational eye movements on the response of neurons in area MT of the macaque , 1998, Visual Neuroscience.

[62]  D. Snodderly,et al.  Studying striate cortex neurons in behaving monkeys: Benefits of image stabilization , 1987, Vision Research.

[63]  G. Orban,et al.  Spatial heterogeneity of inhibitory surrounds in the middle temporal visual area. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[64]  T. Freeman,et al.  Path perception and Filehne illusion compared: model and data , 1999, Vision Research.

[65]  A. Wertheim,et al.  Retinal and Extraretinal Information in Movement Perception: How to Invert the Filehne Illusion , 1987, Perception.

[66]  N Osaka,et al.  Difference of Spatial Frequency Selectivity between Static and Flicker Motion Aftereffects , 1994, Perception.

[67]  Salvatore Squatrito,et al.  ‘Real-motion’ cells in the primary visual cortex of macaque monkeys , 1984, Brain Research.

[68]  Martin S. Banks,et al.  Perceived head-centric speed is affected by both extra-retinal and retinal errors , 1998, Vision Research.

[69]  J Allman,et al.  Direction- and Velocity-Specific Responses from beyond the Classical Receptive Field in the Middle Temporal Visual Area (MT) , 1985, Perception.

[70]  J W Belliveau,et al.  Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 1995, Science.

[71]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  A jitter after-effect reveals motion-based stabilization of vision , 1998, Nature.

[72]  D. B. Bender,et al.  Global visual processing in the monkey superior colliculus , 1986, Brain Research.

[73]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Visual thresholds for shearing motion in monkey and man , 1985, Vision Research.

[74]  R. Born,et al.  Segregation of global and local motion processing in primate middle temporal visual area , 1993, Nature.

[75]  D. B. Bender,et al.  Loss of relative-motion sensitivity in the monkey superior colliculus after lesions of cortical area MT , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[76]  Henry Kennedy,et al.  Functional implications of the anatomical organization of the callosal projections of visual areas V1 and V2 in the macaque monkey , 1988, Behavioural Brain Research.