Positive Affect Is Associated With Reduced Fixation in a Realistic Medical Simulation

Objective: This study extends previous research by exploring the association between mood states (i.e., positive and negative affect) and fixation in practicing anesthetists using a realistic medical simulation. Background: The impact of practitioner emotional states on fixation is a neglected area of research. Emerging evidence is demonstrating the role of positive affect in facilitating problem solving and innovation, with demonstrated implications for practitioner fixation. Method: Twelve practicing anesthetists (4 females; Mage= 39 years; SD = 6.71) were involved in a medical simulation. Prior to the simulation, practitioners rated the frequency they had experienced various positive and negative emotions in the previous three days. During the simulation, the patient deteriorated rapidly, and anesthetists were observed for their degree of fixation. After the simulation, practitioners indicated the frequency of these same emotions during the simulation. Results: Nonparametric correlations were used to explore the independent relationships between positive and negative affect and the behavioral measures. Only positive affect impacted the likelihood of fixation. Anesthetists who reported more frequent recent positive affect in the three days prior to the simulation and during the simulation tended to be less fixated as judged by independent raters, identified a decline in patient oxygen saturation more quickly, and more rapidly implemented the necessary intervention (surgical cricothyroidotomy). Conclusion: These findings have some real-world implications for positive affect in patient safety. Application: This research has broad implications for professions where fixation may impair practice. This research suggests that professional training should teach practitioners to identify their emotions and understand the role of these emotions in fixation.

[1]  R. Glavin,et al.  No simple fix for fixation errors: cognitive processes and their clinical applications , 2010, Anaesthesia.

[2]  Gilles Pourtois,et al.  Neural systems for orienting attention to the location of threat signals: An event-related fMRI study , 2006, NeuroImage.

[3]  J. Henry,et al.  The positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS): construct validity, measurement properties and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. , 2004, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[4]  A. Isen,et al.  Positive Affect Facilitates Integration of Information and Decreases Anchoring in Reasoning among Physicians , 1997 .

[5]  B. Löwe,et al.  A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: the GAD-7. , 2006, Archives of internal medicine.

[6]  R Flin,et al.  Beyond monitoring: distributed situation awareness in anaesthesia. , 2010, British journal of anaesthesia.

[7]  Robert H. Somers,et al.  A new asymmetric measure of association for ordinal variables. , 1962 .

[8]  R. Spitzer,et al.  The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. , 2001, Journal of general internal medicine.

[9]  D. Watson,et al.  Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  Christian E. Waugh,et al.  Smile to see the forest: Facially expressed positive emotions broaden cognition , 2010, Cognition & emotion.

[11]  Penelope M Sanderson,et al.  Auditory displays in anesthesiology , 2009, Current opinion in anaesthesiology.

[12]  E. Fox,et al.  Facial Expressions of Emotion: Are Angry Faces Detected More Efficiently? , 2000, Cognition & emotion.

[13]  D. Clark,et al.  Implementing NICE guidelines for the psychological treatment of depression and anxiety disorders: The IAPT experience , 2011, International review of psychiatry.

[14]  B. Fredrickson,et al.  The Undoing Effect of Positive Emotions , 2000, Motivation and emotion.

[15]  D. Gaba,et al.  The Response of Anesthesia Trainees to Simulated Critical Incidents , 1988, Anesthesia and analgesia.

[16]  David M. Gaba,et al.  Simulation-Based Training in Anesthesia Crisis Resource Management (ACRM): A Decade of Experience , 2001 .

[17]  Frank Drews,et al.  The Right Picture Is Worth a Thousand Numbers: Data Displays in Anesthesia , 2006, Hum. Factors.

[18]  Julius Kuhl,et al.  Positive Affect and Flexibility: Overcoming the Precedence of Global over Local Processing of Visual Information , 2005 .

[19]  B. Fredrickson The role of positive emotions in positive psychology. The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. , 2001, The American psychologist.

[20]  J. Henderson,et al.  Difficult Airway Society guidelines for management of the unanticipated difficult intubation , 2004, Anaesthesia.

[21]  Christopher M. Janelle,et al.  Where’s the Emotion? How Sport Psychology Can Inform Research on Emotion in Human Factors , 2011 .

[22]  Denis Besnard,et al.  Expert error in trouble-shooting: an exploratory study in electronics , 1999, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[23]  Min Cheol Whang,et al.  Preparing Computers for Affective Communication: A Psychophysiological Concept and Preliminary Results , 2003, Hum. Factors.

[24]  Penelope M Sanderson,et al.  Monitoring with Head-Mounted Displays: Performance and Safety in a Full-Scale Simulator and Part-Task Trainer , 2009, Anesthesia and analgesia.

[25]  Matthew B. Weinger,et al.  Ergonomic and Human Factors Affecting Anesthetic Vigilance and Monitoring Performance in the Operating Room Environment , 1990, Anesthesiology.