The Goal Circuit Model: A Hierarchical Multi-Route Model of the Acquisition and Control of Routine Sequential Action in Humans
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Tirin Moore,et al. A map of complex movements in motor cortex of primates , 2004 .
[2] T. Shallice,et al. Hierarchical schemas and goals in the control of sequential behavior. , 2006, Psychological review.
[3] Ronald J. Williams,et al. Gradient-based learning algorithms for recurrent networks and their computational complexity , 1995 .
[4] Isabelle Rouleau,et al. Frontal lesions impair the attentional control of movements during motor learning , 1999, Neuropsychologia.
[5] Christian Montag,et al. Imaging the structure of the human anxious brain: a review of findings from neuroscientific personality psychology , 2013, Reviews in the neurosciences.
[6] Michael W. Montgomery,et al. Naturalistic action impairment in closed head injury. , 1998, Neuropsychology.
[7] James L. McClelland,et al. Generalization with Componential Attractors: Word and Nonword Reading in an Attractor Network , 1993 .
[8] D. Norman,et al. Attention to Action: Willed and Automatic Control of Behavior Technical Report No. 8006. , 1980 .
[9] H. Heckhausen,et al. Intentional action and action slips. , 1990 .
[10] E. Miller,et al. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.
[11] T. Shallice,et al. The origins of utilization behaviour. , 1989, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[12] J. Gregory Trafton,et al. Memory for goals: an activation-based model , 2002, Cogn. Sci..
[13] Tim Shallice,et al. The domain of supervisory processes and the temporal organisation of behaviour , 1998 .
[14] W. N. Schoenfeld,et al. Principles of Psychology , 2007 .
[15] M. Land,et al. The Roles of Vision and Eye Movements in the Control of Activities of Daily Living , 1998, Perception.
[16] Daniel Dennett,et al. Language and thought: Reflections on language and mind , 1998 .
[17] D. Glasspool. The Integration and Control of Behaviour: Insights from Neuroscience and AI , 2005 .
[18] 三嶋 博之. The theory of affordances , 2008 .
[19] Denis Mareschal,et al. Action selection in complex routinized sequential behaviors. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[20] Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al. Anatomy of motor learning. I. Frontal cortex and attention to action. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.
[21] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. The organization of sequential actions , 2000 .
[22] A. Battersby. Plans and the Structure of Behavior , 1968 .
[23] Umberto Castiello,et al. A neuroimaging study of selection-for-action: A reach-to-grasp study , 2004 .
[24] Tim Shallice,et al. Fractionation of the supervisory system. , 2002 .
[25] Michael W. Montgomery,et al. The quantitative description of action disorganisation after brain damage: a case study , 1991 .
[26] Zoubin Ghahramani,et al. Computational principles of movement neuroscience , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[27] D. Norman. Categorization of action slips. , 1981 .
[28] Richard P. Cooper,et al. A Systematic Methodology for Cognitive Modelling , 1996, Artif. Intell..
[29] Richard J. Brown. Neuropsychology Mental Structure , 1989 .
[30] Tim Shallice,et al. Contrasting domains in the control of action: the routine and the non-routine. , 2005 .
[31] F. Lhermitte. 'Utilization behaviour' and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[32] D. Mareschal,et al. A REINFORCEMENT MODEL OF SEQUENTIAL ROUTINE ACTION , 2005 .
[33] D. Plaut,et al. Such stuff as habits are made on: A reply to Cooper and Shallice (2006). , 2006 .
[34] Lauren M. Bylsma,et al. Distraction and action slips in an everyday task: Evidence for a dynamic representation of task context , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[35] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. On the Control of Control: The Role of Dopamine in Regulating Prefrontal Function and Working Memory , 2007 .
[36] E. Koechlin,et al. The Architecture of Cognitive Control in the Human Prefrontal Cortex , 2003, Science.
[37] Terence W. Picton,et al. Mapping Task Switching in Frontal Cortex Through Neuropsychological Group Studies , 2008, Front. Neurosci..
[38] G. Miller,et al. Plans and the structure of behavior , 1960 .
[39] T. Shallice,et al. CONTENTION SCHEDULING AND THE CONTROL OF ROUTINE ACTIVITIES , 2000, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[40] D. Plaut,et al. Doing without schema hierarchies: a recurrent connectionist approach to normal and impaired routine sequential action. , 2004, Psychological review.
[41] Rodney A. Brooks,et al. How to Build Complete Creatures Rather than Isolated Cognitive Simulators , 2014 .
[42] J. Shaoul. Human Error , 1973, Nature.
[43] G. Goldberg,et al. Medial frontal cortex infarction and the alien hand sign. , 1981, Archives of neurology.
[44] M. Conway. Verifying autobiographical facts , 1987, Cognition.
[45] A. Dickinson. Actions and habits: the development of behavioural autonomy , 1985 .
[46] David Glasspool,et al. Learning Action Affordances and Action Schemas , 2000, NCPW.
[47] Raj M. Ratwani,et al. A memory for goals model of sequence errors , 2011, Cognitive Systems Research.
[48] L. Buxbaum,et al. The Coffee Challenge: A new method for the study of everyday action errors , 2007, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.
[49] D. Brooks,et al. Motor sequence learning: a study with positron emission tomography , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[50] J. Duncan. Disorganisation of behaviour after frontal lobe damage , 1986 .
[51] M. Botvinick,et al. Hierarchically organized behavior and its neural foundations: A reinforcement learning perspective , 2009, Cognition.
[52] James B. Rowe,et al. Prefrontal cortex and attention to action , 2005 .
[53] D. Spalding. The Principles of Psychology , 1873, Nature.
[54] A. Luria. Higher Cortical Functions in Man , 1980, Springer US.
[55] T Shallice,et al. The domain of supervisory processes and temporal organization of behaviour. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[56] G. Humphreys,et al. Visual affordances direct action: neuropsychological evidence from manual interference. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[57] G. Humphreys,et al. Disordered action schema and action disorganisation syndrome , 1998 .
[58] Bernhard Hommel,et al. Planning and Representing Intentional Action , 2003, TheScientificWorldJournal.
[59] P. Dayan,et al. Uncertainty-based competition between prefrontal and dorsolateral striatal systems for behavioral control , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[60] Donald G. MacKay,et al. The organization of perception and action. A theory for language and other cognitive skills , 1987, The Italian Journal of Neurological Sciences.
[61] Jordan Grafman,et al. Procedural learning is impaired in patients with prefrontal lesions , 1999, Neurology.
[62] J. Reason. Actions not as planned : The price of automatisation , 1979 .
[63] T. Shallice,et al. The Organisation of Mind , 2011, Cortex.
[64] H. Spinnler,et al. Right-sided anarchic (alien) hand: A longitudinal study , 1991, Neuropsychologia.
[65] John R. Anderson,et al. The acquisition of robust and flexible cognitive skills. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[66] T. Shallice,et al. The simulation of action disorganisation in complex activities of daily living , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[67] Pat Langley,et al. Ability, Breadth, and Parsimony in Computational Models of Higher-Order Cognition , 2008, Cogn. Sci..
[68] B. Balleine,et al. The Role of Learning in the Operation of Motivational Systems , 2002 .
[69] David T. Neal,et al. A new look at habits and the habit-goal interface. , 2007, Psychological review.
[70] John R. Anderson,et al. From recurrent choice to skill learning: a reinforcement-learning model. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[71] Richard S. Sutton,et al. Reinforcement Learning: An Introduction , 1998, IEEE Trans. Neural Networks.