The influence of the magnitude, probability, and valence of potential wins and losses on the amplitude of the feedback negativity.

We assessed the influence of the variables outcome potential, feedback valence, magnitude, and probability on the amplitude of the feedback negativity (FN). Outcome potential was defined as the a priori valence of an upcoming feedback, that is, is there a potential win or potential loss? All these variables have been studied previously, although never together, but the findings have been contradictory. We analyzed the event-related potential (ERP) after feedback presentation in a reinforcement-learning task to examine the effects of all the variables on feedback negativity. Our results show that outcome potential, feedback valence, probability, and magnitude all influence feedback related ERPs. Taken together, the findings suggest that ERPs in the time range of the feedback negativity are primarily driven by positive outcomes (reinforcement) rather than negative outcomes (punishment).

[1]  Anna Weinberg,et al.  Event‐related potential activity in the basal ganglia differentiates rewards from nonrewards: Temporospatial principal components analysis and source localization of the feedback negativity , 2011, Human brain mapping.

[2]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Dissociated roles of the anterior cingulate cortex in reward and conflict processing as revealed by the feedback error-related negativity and N200 , 2011, Biological Psychology.

[3]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Reward positivity elicited by predictive cues , 2011, Neuroreport.

[4]  I. Daum,et al.  It is less than you expected: The feedback-related negativity reflects violations of reward magnitude expectations , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  Agustín Ibáñez,et al.  Size and probability of rewards modulate the feedback error-related negativity associated with wins but not losses in a monetarily rewarded gambling task , 2010, NeuroImage.

[6]  Jutta Kray,et al.  Developmental differences in learning and error processing: evidence from ERPs. , 2009, Psychophysiology.

[7]  Xiaolin Zhou,et al.  The P300 and reward valence, magnitude, and expectancy in outcome evaluation , 2009, Brain Research.

[8]  R. Baker,et al.  When is an error not a prediction error? An electrophysiological investigation , 2009, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[9]  Scott A. Huettel,et al.  Rapid Electrophysiological Brain Responses are Influenced by Both Valence and Magnitude of Monetary Rewards , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[10]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The feedback correct-related positivity: sensitivity of the event-related brain potential to unexpected positive feedback. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[11]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[12]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  It's worse than you thought: the feedback negativity and violations of reward prediction in gambling tasks. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[13]  M. Delgado,et al.  Reward‐Related Responses in the Human Striatum , 2007, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[14]  Johannes Hewig,et al.  Decision-making in Blackjack: an electrophysiological analysis. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Reward expectation modulates feedback-related negativity and EEG spectra , 2007, NeuroImage.

[16]  Jeff T. Larsen,et al.  The good, the bad and the neutral: Electrophysiological responses to feedback stimuli , 2006, Brain Research.

[17]  P. Read Montague,et al.  When Things Are Better or Worse than Expected: The Medial Frontal Cortex and the Allocation of Processing Resources , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The feedback-related negativity reflects the binary evaluation of good versus bad outcomes , 2006, Biological Psychology.

[19]  H. Murohashi,et al.  Discrepancy between feedback negativity and subjective evaluation in gambling , 2005, Neuroreport.

[20]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Knowing good from bad: differential activation of human cortical areas by positive and negative outcomes , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[21]  Atsushi Sato,et al.  Effects of value and reward magnitude on feedback negativity and P300 , 2005, Neuroreport.

[22]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Brain potentials associated with expected and unexpected good and bad outcomes. , 2005, Psychophysiology.

[23]  N. Yeung,et al.  On the ERN and the significance of errors. , 2005, Psychophysiology.

[24]  Tomifusa Kuboki,et al.  Error-related negativity reflects detection of negative reward prediction error , 2004, Neuroreport.

[25]  A. Sanfey,et al.  Independent Coding of Reward Magnitude and Valence in the Human Brain , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Sensitivity of electrophysiological activity from medial frontal cortex to utilitarian and performance feedback. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[27]  Jeff T. Larsen,et al.  Context dependence of the event-related brain potential associated with reward and punishment. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[28]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Errors in reward prediction are re£ected in the event-related brain potential , 2003 .

[29]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Implementation of error-processing in the human anterior cingulate cortex: a source analysis of the magnetic equivalent of the error-related negativity , 2003, Biological Psychology.

[30]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Temporal Difference Models and Reward-Related Learning in the Human Brain , 2003, Neuron.

[31]  W. Schultz Getting Formal with Dopamine and Reward , 2002, Neuron.

[32]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.

[33]  Markus Kiefer,et al.  Human anterior cingulate cortex is activated by negative feedback: evidence from event-related potentials in a guessing task , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.

[34]  Adrian R. Willoughby,et al.  The Medial Frontal Cortex and the Rapid Processing of Monetary Gains and Losses , 2002, Science.

[35]  W. Schultz Multiple reward signals in the brain , 2000, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[36]  W. Schultz,et al.  Relative reward preference in primate orbitofrontal cortex , 1999, Nature.

[37]  H. Jasper,et al.  The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. The International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. , 1999, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[38]  C. Braun,et al.  Event-Related Brain Potentials Following Incorrect Feedback in a Time-Estimation Task: Evidence for a Generic Neural System for Error Detection , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[39]  Peter Dayan,et al.  A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward , 1997, Science.

[40]  M. Coles,et al.  "Where did I go wrong?" A psychophysiological analysis of error detection. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[41]  D. Meyer,et al.  A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .

[42]  E Donchin,et al.  A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[43]  E Donchin,et al.  Event-related brain potentials and subjective probability in a learning task , 1980, Memory & cognition.

[44]  E Donchin,et al.  P300 and stimulus categorization: two plus one is not so different from one plus one. , 1980, Psychophysiology.

[45]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect Theory : An Analysis of Decision under Risk Author ( s ) : , 2007 .

[46]  S A Hillyard,et al.  P3 waves to the discrimination of targets in homogeneous and heterogeneous stimulus sequences. , 1977, Psychophysiology.

[47]  E. Donchin,et al.  On quantifying surprise: the variation of event-related potentials with subjective probability. , 1977, Psychophysiology.

[48]  N. Squires,et al.  The effect of stimulus sequence on the waveform of the cortical event-related potential. , 1976, Science.