Feedback delay attenuates implicit but facilitates explicit adjustments to a visuomotor rotation

&NA; We examined the effects of delaying terminal visual feedback on the relative contribution of explicit and implicit components of adaptation to a visuomotor rotation. Participants practiced a 30° rotation while receiving terminal visual feedback with either a short (0 ms), medium (200 ms), or long (1500 ms) delay. Explicit and implicit adjustments were dissociated by a series of posttests. While overall adaptation did not differ significantly between groups, aftereffects progressively decreased with increasing feedback delay. Moreover, explicit knowledge of the rotation increased in both the medium and high delay groups relative to the short delay group, but did not differ between the former two. This finding of feedback delay differentially affecting implicit adjustments as indexed by aftereffects and conscious strategic corrections based on explicit knowledge of the transformation substantiates the importance of distinguishing implicit and explicit components of adaptation even with rotations of smaller size and emphasizes the need to consider time delays in the interpretation of adaptation experiments and potentially in the design of training environments. HighlightsDelaying visual feedback facilitates explicit learning of a visuomotor rotation.Implicit learning is attenuated by the feedback delay.Implicit learning decreased progressively across two delays, 200 and 1500 ms.Explicit learning showed a similar increase with 200 as with 1500 ms delay.

[1]  Ethan R. Buch,et al.  Visuomotor adaptation in normal aging. , 2003, Learning & memory.

[2]  Nils Bender,et al.  Das Lateral Preference Inventory: Itemhomogenität der deutschen Version , 2009 .

[3]  D. Halpern,et al.  Sex differences in intelligence. Implications for education. , 1997, The American psychologist.

[4]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Spatio-Temporal Prediction Modulates the Perception of Self-Produced Stimuli , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[5]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Relevance of error: what drives motor adaptation? , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  D. Willingham,et al.  Deficit in learning of a motor skill requiring strategy, but not of perceptuomotor recalibration, with aging. , 1998, Learning & memory.

[7]  A. Haith,et al.  Model-based and model-free mechanisms of human motor learning. , 2013, Advances in experimental medicine and biology.

[8]  Richard B. Ivry,et al.  Flexible Cognitive Strategies during Motor Learning , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[9]  R. Ivry,et al.  Cerebellar and prefrontal cortex contributions to adaptation, strategies, and reinforcement learning. , 2014, Progress in brain research.

[10]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Estimating the sources of motor errors for adaptation and generalization , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[11]  J. J. Siegel,et al.  Persistent activity in a cortical-to-subcortical circuit: bridging the temporal gap in trace eyelid conditioning. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  Britne A. Shabbott,et al.  Learning a visuomotor rotation: simultaneous visual and proprioceptive information is crucial for visuomotor remapping , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  W. Snow,et al.  Sex differences among non-brain-damaged adults on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scales: a review of the literature. , 1990, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[14]  Stephan Riek,et al.  Real-time error detection but not error correction drives automatic visuomotor adaptation , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  Mathias Hegele,et al.  Adaptation to visuomotor rotations in younger and older adults. , 2008, Psychology and aging.

[16]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Learning from Sensory and Reward Prediction Errors during Motor Adaptation , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[17]  Krista M Bond,et al.  Flexible explicit but rigid implicit learning in a visuomotor adaptation task. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  Susanne M. Jaeggi,et al.  Author's Personal Copy Behavioural Brain Research the Effects of Working Memory Resource Depletion and Training on Sensorimotor Adaptation , 2022 .

[19]  D. Nozaki,et al.  Adaptation to Visual Feedback Delay Influences Visuomotor Learning , 2012, PloS one.

[20]  Waka Fujisaki,et al.  Effects of Delayed Visual Feedback on Grooved Pegboard Test Performance , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[21]  J. Wallis,et al.  The Role of Prefrontal Cortex in Working Memory: A Mini Review , 2015, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[22]  Masao Ito,et al.  Stimulus parameters for induction of long-term depression in in vitro rat Purkinje cells , 1994, Neuroscience Research.

[23]  R. Ivry,et al.  Delayed feedback during sensorimotor learning selectively disrupts adaptation but not strategy use. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  J. Krakauer,et al.  An Implicit Plan Overrides an Explicit Strategy during Visuomotor Adaptation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  S. Kitazawa,et al.  Prism Adaptation of Reaching Movements: Specificity for the Velocity of Reaching , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Effective reinforcement learning following cerebellar damage requires a balance between exploration and motor noise , 2015, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[27]  Marc Jeannerod,et al.  Preserved Adjustment but Impaired Awareness in a Sensory-Motor Conflict following Prefrontal Lesions , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[28]  R. Masters Knowledge, knerves and know-how: The role of explicit versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of a complex motor skill under pressure , 1992 .

[29]  M. Kano,et al.  Stimulation parameters influencing climbing fibre induced long-term depression of parallel fibre synapses , 1989, Neuroscience Research.

[30]  Hilde Haider,et al.  The route from implicit learning to verbal expression of what has been learned , 2003 .

[31]  Rachael D. Seidler,et al.  Contributions of Spatial Working Memory to Visuomotor Learning , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  D M Wolpert,et al.  Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control , 1998, Neural Networks.

[33]  Mathias Hegele,et al.  Explicit and implicit components of visuo-motor adaptation: An analysis of individual differences , 2015, Consciousness and Cognition.

[34]  H. Heuer,et al.  The Impact of Augmented Information on Visuo-Motor Adaptation in Younger and Older Adults , 2010, PloS one.

[35]  Juan Fernandez-Ruiz,et al.  The effect of Parkinson's disease and Huntington's disease on human visuomotor learning , 2013, The European journal of neuroscience.

[36]  Darius E. Parvin,et al.  Mirror Reversal and Visual Rotation Are Learned and Consolidated via Separate Mechanisms: Recalibrating or Learning De Novo? , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  R. Clark,et al.  Classical conditioning and brain systems: the role of awareness. , 1998, Science.

[38]  G. Abbruzzese,et al.  The rationale for motor learning in Parkinson's disease. , 2009, European journal of physical and rehabilitation medicine.

[39]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Explicit and Implicit Contributions to Learning in a Sensorimotor Adaptation Task , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  Masaya Hirashima,et al.  Habituation to Feedback Delay Restores Degraded Visuomotor Adaptation by Altering Both Sensory Prediction Error and the Sensitivity of Adaptation to the Error , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[41]  E. Vaucher,et al.  Activation of the mouse primary visual cortex by medial prefrontal subregion stimulation is not mediated by cholinergic basalo-cortical projections , 2015, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[42]  Vincent S. Huang,et al.  Rethinking Motor Learning and Savings in Adaptation Paradigms: Model-Free Memory for Successful Actions Combines with Internal Models , 2011, Neuron.

[43]  M. Hinder,et al.  The contribution of visual feedback to visuomotor adaptation: How much and when? , 2008, Brain Research.

[44]  David M. Huberdeau,et al.  The Influence of Movement Preparation Time on the Expression of Visuomotor Learning and Savings , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  Konrad P. Körding,et al.  Estimating the Relevance of World Disturbances to Explain Savings, Interference and Long-Term Motor Adaptation Effects , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[46]  Kevin Shockley,et al.  Progress in Motor Control , 2013, Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology.

[47]  R. Masters,et al.  The role of working memory in motor learning and performance , 2003, Consciousness and Cognition.

[48]  Amir Karniel,et al.  Knowledge of Performance is Insufficient for Implicit Visuomotor Rotation Adaptation , 2012, Journal of motor behavior.

[49]  R. Held,et al.  Adaptation to displaced and delayed visual feedback from the hand. , 1966 .

[50]  R. French,et al.  Implicit learning and consciousness: A graded, dynamic perspective , 2002 .

[51]  Kurt A. Thoroughman,et al.  Motor Adaptation Scaled by the Difficulty of a Secondary Cognitive Task , 2008, PloS one.

[52]  Shigeru Kitazawa,et al.  Prism adaptation with delayed visual error signals in the monkey , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[53]  Romeo Chua,et al.  Is proprioception calibrated during visually guided movements? , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[54]  S. Kitazawa,et al.  Effects of delayed visual information on the rate and amount of prism adaptation in the human , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[55]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Is the cerebellum a smith predictor? , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[56]  H. Heuer,et al.  Age-related variations of visuomotor adaptation result from both the acquisition and the application of explicit knowledge. , 2013, Psychology and aging.

[57]  Wolfgang Taube,et al.  Online and post-trial feedback differentially affect implicit adaptation to a visuomotor rotation , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.

[58]  Philip N. Sabes,et al.  Modeling Sensorimotor Learning with Linear Dynamical Systems , 2006, Neural Computation.

[59]  J. Churchwell,et al.  Medial prefrontal cortex and pavlovian conditioning: trace versus delay conditioning. , 2002, Behavioral neuroscience.